Forensic Psychology

Home > Other > Forensic Psychology > Page 33
Forensic Psychology Page 33

by Graham M Davies


  Hyman, I. E., & Loftus, E. F. (2002). False childhood memories and eyewitness memory errors. In M. L. Eisen, J. A. Quas , & G. S. Goodman (Eds.), Memory and suggestibility in the forensic interview (pp. 63–84). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.

  Kebbell, M. R., & Gilchrist, E. A. (2010). Eliciting evidence from eyewitnesses for court proceedings. In J. R. Adler & J. M. Gray (Eds.), Forensic psychology: Concepts, debates and practice (pp. 145–161). UK: Willan Publishing.

  Kebbell, M. R., & Milne, R. (1998). Police officers’ perceptions of eyewitness performance in forensic investigations. The Journal of Social Psychology, 138, 323–330.

  Kebbell, M. R., & Wagstaff, G. F. (1999). The effectiveness of the cognitive interview. In D. Canter & L. Allison (Eds.), Interviewing and deception (pp. 25–39). Aldershot: Ashgate Publishing.

  Koriat, A., & Goldsmith, M. (1994). Memory in naturalistic and laboratory contexts: distinguishing the accuracy-oriented and quantity-oriented approaches to memory assessment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 297–315.

  Lamb, M. E., Orbach, Y., Warren, A. R., Esplin, P. W., & Hershkowitz, I. (2006). Getting the most out of children: Factors affecting the informativeness of young witnesses. In: M. P. Toglia, J. D. Read, D. F. Ross , & R. C. L. Lindsay (Eds.), Handbook of eyewitness psychology Vol 1: Memory for events (pp. 423–446). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

  Laney, C., & Loftus, E. F. (2010). Change blindness and eyewitness testimony. In G. M. Davies & D. B. Wright (Eds.), Current issues in applied memory research (pp. 142–159). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

  Levin, D. T., Simons, D. J., Angelone, B. L., & Chabris, C. F. (2002). Memory for centrally attended changing objects in an incidental real-world change detection paradigm. British Journal of Psychology, 93, 289–302.

  Li, S. C., Naveh-Benjamin, M., & Lindenberger, U. (2005). Aging neuromodulation impairs associative binding neurocomputational account. Psychological Science, 16, 445–450.

  Lindsay, D. S., & Johnson, M. K. (1987). Reality monitoring and suggestibility: Children’s ability to discriminate among memories from different sources. In S. J. Ceci, M. P. Toglia , & D. F. Ross (Eds.), Children’s eyewitness memory (pp. 92–121). New York: Springer.

  Loftus, E. F. (1979). The malleability of human memory: Information introduced after we view an incident can transform memory. American Scientist, 67, 312–320.

  Loftus, E. F., & Hoffman, H. G. (1989). Misinformation and memory: The creation of new memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 100–104.

  Loftus, E. F., Miller, D. G., & Burns, H. J. (1978). Semantic integration of verbal information into a visual memory. Journal of experimental psychology: Human learning and memory, 4, 19–31.

  Loftus, E. F., & Palmer, J. C. (1974). Reconstruction of automobile destruction: An example of the interaction between language and memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 585–589.

  Loftus, E. F., & Pickrell, J. E. (1995). The formation of false memories. Psychiatric annals, 25, 720–725.

  Loftus, E. F., Schooler, J. W., & Wagenaar, W. A. (1985). The fate of memory: Comment on McCloskey and Zaragoza. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 375–380.

  Luus, C. A., & Wells, G. L. (1994). The malleability of eyewitness confidence: Co-witness and perseverance effects. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 714–723.

  McCloskey, M., & Zaragoza, M. (1985a). Misleading post event information and memory for events: Arguments and evidence against memory impairment hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 1–16.

  McCloskey, M., & Zaragoza, M. (1985b). Postevent information and memory: Reply to Loftus, Schooler, and Wagenaar. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 381–387.

  Memon, A., & Vartoukian, R. (1996). The effects of repeated questioning on young children’s eyewitness testimony. British Journal of Psychology, 87, 403–415.

  Morgan, C. A., Southwick, S., Steffian, G., Hazlett, G. A., & Loftus, E. F. (2013). Misinformation can influence memory for recently experienced, highly stressful events. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 36, 11–17.

  Münsterberg, H. (1908). On the witness stand. New York: Clark, Boardman.

  National Police Chiefs’ Council (2015). Over 1400 suspects investigated for child sexual abuse by people of public prominence, or within institutions. Retrieved from http://news.npcc.police.uk/releases/over-1400-suspects-investigated-for-child-sexual-abuse-by-people-of-public- prominence-or-within-institutions

  Neisser, U. (1982). Memory: What are the important questions? In U. Neisser (Ed.), Memory observed (pp. 3–12). San Francisco: Freeman

  Office of National Statistics. (2011/2012). Focus on: violent crime & sexual offences. Retrieved from http://www.ons.gov.uk/ons/dcp171778_298904.pdf

  Ofshe, R. & Watters, E. (1996). Making monsters: False memories, psychotherapy, and sexual hysteria. Oakland CA: University of California Press.

  Operation Yewtree (n.d.) In Wikipedia. Retrieved from https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Operation_Yewtree

  Overman, A. A., Wiseman, K. D., Allison, M., & Stephens, J. D. (2013). Age differences and schema effects in memory for crime information. Experimental Aging Research, 39, 215–234.

  Palmer, F., Flowe, H. D., Takarangi, M. K., & Humphries, J. E. (2013). Intoxicated witnesses and suspects: An archival analysis of their involvement in criminal case processing. Law and Human Behavior, 37, 54–59.

  Paterson, H. M., & Kemp, R. I. (2006). Co-witnesses talk: A survey of eyewitness discussion. Psychology, Crime & Law, 12, 181–191.

  Peters, M. J., Jelicic, M., & Merckelbach, H. (2006). When stereotypes backfire: Trying to suppress stereotypes produces false recollections of a crime. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 11, 327–336.

  Pezdek, K., Finger, K., & Hodge, D. (1997). Planting false childhood memories: The role of event plausibility. Psychological Science, 8, 437–441.

  Pezdek, K., & Hodge, D. (1999). Planting false childhood memories in children: The role of event plausibility. Child Development, 70, 887–895.

  Pickel, K. L. (1998). Unusualness and threat as possible causes of weapon focus. Memory, 6, 277–295.

  Pickel, K. L. (1999). The influence of context on the “weapon focus” effect. Law and Human Behavior, 23, 299–311.

  Pickel, K. L., Ross, S. J., & Truelove, R. S. (2006). Do weapons automatically capture attention? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 871–893.

  Poole, D. A., & White, L. T. (1991). Effects of question repetition on the eyewitness testimony of children and adults. Developmental Psychology, 27, 975–986.

  Pope, H. G., Jr., Oliva, P. S., & Hudson, J. I. (1999). The scientific status of research on repressed memories. In D. L. Faigman, D. H. Kaye, M. J. Saks , & J. Sanders (Eds.), Modem scientific evidence: The law and science of expert testimony (Vol. 1, pp. 115–155). St. Paul, MN: West Group.

  Porter, S., & Birt, A. R. (2001). Is traumatic memory special? A comparison of traumatic memory characteristics with memory for other emotional life experiences. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, S101–S117.

  Porter, S., Campbell, M. A., Birt, A. R., & Woodworth, M. T. (2003). “He said, she said”: A psychological perspective on historical memory evidence in the courtroom. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 44, 190–206.

  Read, J. D., & Lindsay, D. S. (2000). “Amnesia” for summer camps and high school graduation: Memory work increases reports of prior periods of remembering less. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 13, 129–147.

  Reisberg, D., & Heuer, F. (2006). The influence of emotion on memory in forensic settings. In M. P. Toglia, J. D. Read, D. F. Ross , & R. C. L. Lindsay (Eds.), Handbook of eyewitness psychology: Memory for events (Vol. 1). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

  Safer, M. A., Christianson, S. Å., Autry, M. W., & Österlund, K. (1998). Tunnel memory for traumatic events. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12, 99–117.

  Schreiber Compo , N., Evans, J. R., Carol, R. N., Kemp , D., Villalba , D., Ham, L. S., & Ros
e , S. (2011). Alcohol intoxication and memory for events: A snapshot of alcohol myopia in a real-world drinking scenario. Memory, 19, 202–210.

  Scrivner, E., & Safer, M. A. (1988). Eyewitnesses show hyperamnesia for details about a violent event. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 371–377.

  Searcy, J. H., Bartlett, J. C., Memon, A., & Swanson, K. (2001). Aging and lineup performance at long retention intervals: Effects of metamemory and context reinstatement. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 207–214.

  Shaw, J. S. III, & Zerr, T. K. (2003). Extra effort during memory retrieval may be associated with increases in eyewitness confidence. Law and Human Behavior, 27, 315–329.

  Skagerberg, E. M., & Wright, D. B. (2008). The prevalence of co-witnesses and co-witness discussions in real eyewitnesses. Psychology, Crime & Law, 14, 513–521.

  Smart, S. M., Berry, M. A., & Rodriguez, D. N. (2014). Skilled observation and change blindness: A comparison of law enforcement and student samples. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28, 590–596.

  Steele, C. M., & Josephs, R. A. (1990). Alcohol myopia: Its prized and dangerous effects. American Psychologist, 45, 921–933.

  Steele, C. M., & Southwick, L. (1985). Alcohol and social behavior: I. The psychology of drunken excess. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 18–34.

  Terr, L. C. (1979). Children of Chowchilla: A study of psychic trauma. The Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 34, 547–623.

  Terr, L. C. (1983). Chowchilla revisited: The effects of psychic trauma four years after a school-bus kidnapping. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 1543–1550.

  Thompson, J., Morton, J., & Fraser, L. (1997). Memories for the Marchioness. Memory, 5, 615–638.

  Tinti, C., Schmidt, S., Testa, S., & Levine, L. J. (2014). Distinct processes shape flashbulb and event memories. Memory & Cognition, 42, 539–551.

  Tuckey, M. R., & Brewer, N. (2003a). The influence of schemas, stimulus ambiguity, and interview schedule on eyewitness memory over time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 9, 101–118.

  Tuckey, M. R., & Brewer, N. (2003b). How schemas affect eyewitness memory over repeated retrieval attempts. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 785–800.

  Valentine, T., & Mesout, J. (2009). Eyewitness identification under stress in the London Dungeon. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 151–161.

  Van Knippenberg, A. D., Dijksterhuis, A. P., & Vermeulen , D. (1999). Judgement and memory of a criminal act: The effects of stereotypes and cognitive load. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 191–201.

  van Koppen , P., & Lochun , S. (1997). Portraying perpetrators: The validity of offender descriptions by witnesses. Law and Human Behavior, 21, 661–685.

  Vredeveldt, A., Hitch, G. J., & Baddeley, A. D. (2011). Eye-closure helps memory by reducing cognitive load and enhancing visualisation. Memory & Cognition, 39, 1253–1263.

  Wade, K. A., Garry, M., Read, J. D., & Lindsay, S. (2002). A picture is worth a thousand lies: Using false photographs to create false childhood memories. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 597–603.

  Wagenaar, W. A., & Groeneweg, J. (1990). The memory of concentration camp survivors. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 4, 77–87.

  Wagstaff, G. F., MacVeigh, J., Boston, R., Scott, L., Brunas-Wagstaff, J., & Cole, J. (2003). Can laboratory findings on eyewitness testimony be generalized to the real world? An archival analysis of the influence of violence, weapon presence, and age on eyewitness accuracy. Journal of Psychology, 137, 17–28.

  Warren, A. R., Hulse-Trotter, K., & Tubbs, E. (1991). Inducing resistance to suggestibility in children. Law and Human Behavior, 15, 273–285.

  Watson, J. M., Bunting, M. F., Poole, B. J., & Conway, A. A. (2005). Individual differences in susceptibility to false memory in the Deese-Roediger-McDermott Paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 76–85.

  Weber, N., & Brewer, N. (2008). Eyewitness recall: Regulation of grain size and the role of confidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14, 50.

  Wells, G. L. (1978). Applied eyewitness-testimony research: System variables and estimator variables. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1546–1557.

  Wells, G. L., & Olson, E. A. (2003). Eyewitness testimony. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 277–295.

  Williamson, P., Weber, N., & Robertson, M. (2013). The effect of expertise on memory conformity: A test of informational influence. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 31, 607–623.

  Wixted, J. T., & Ebbesen, E. B. (1991). On the form of forgetting. Psychological science, 2, 409–415.

  Wixted, J. T., Mickes, L., Clark, S. E., Gronlund, S. D., & Roediger, H. L. (2015). Initial confidence reliably predicts eyewitness identification accuracy. American Psychologist, 70, 515–526.

  Wright, D. B. (1993). Recall of the Hillsborough disaster over time: Systematic biases of “flashbulb” memories. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 7, 129–138.

  Wright, D. B., Gabbert, F., Memon, A., & London, K. (2008). Changing the criterion for memory conformity in free recall and recognition. Memory, 16, 137–148.

  Wright, D. B., & Loftus, E. F. (1998). How misinformation alters memories. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 71, 155–164.

  Wright, D. B., Memon, A., Skagerberg, E. M., & Gabbert, F. (2009). When eyewitnesses talk. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 174–178.

  Wright, D. B., & Villalba, D. K. (2012). Memory conformity affects inaccurate memories more than accurate memories. Memory, 20, 254–265.

  Yarmey, A. D. (2001). Expert testimony: Does eyewitness memory research have probative value for the courts? Canadian Psychology, 42, 92–100.

  Yerkes, R. M., & J. D. Dodson. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology, 1, 459–482.

  Yuille, J. C., & Cutshall, J. L. (1986). A case study of eyewitness memory of a crime. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71, 291–301.

  Yuille, J. C., & Tollestrup, P. A. (1990). Some effects of alcohol on eyewitness memory. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 268–273.

  Zaragoza, M. S., McCloskey, M., & Jamis, M. (1987). Misleading post event information and recall of the original event: Further evidence against the memory impairment hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 36–44.

  7 Interviewing Witnesses

  ALLISON P. MUGNO, LINDSAY C. MALLOY AND DAVID J. LA ROOY

  CHAPTER OUTLINE

  7.1 INTRODUCTION

  7.2 SHORTCOMINGS AND CONSEQUENCES OF TRADITIONAL INVESTIGATIVE INTERVIEWS

  7.3 THE COGNITIVE INTERVIEW (CI) 7.3.1 Overview of the CI

  7.3.2 Empirical Support for the CI

  7.3.3 Variants of the CI

  7.3.4 Concluding Remarks on the CI

  7.4 INTERVIEWING VULNERABLE WITNESSES 7.4.1 Child Witnesses

  7.4.2 Memorandum of Good Practice (MOGP)

  7.4.3 Achieving Best Evidence (ABE)

  7.4.4 The U.S. National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) Investigative Interview Protocol

  7.4.5 Elderly Witnesses

  7.4.6 Witnesses with Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities

  7.5 SUMMARY

  LEARNING OUTCOMES

  BY THE END OF THIS CHAPTER, YOU SHOULD BE ABLE TO:

  Understand how a psychological analysis of investigative interviewing techniques can inform legal practice

  Appreciate the cognitive and social factors that influence the development of effective interviewing methods for various vulnerable populations

  Understand the implications of developing empirically-based interviewing techniques for improving the event recall of eyewitnesses.

  7.1 INTRODUCTION

  Imagine that you are at the pub with your friends. You start to hear a commotion at the next table. A fight has broken out between two fellow patrons, and it escalates quickly. Above the laughter and music, you hear punches being thrown and glasses
breaking. You notice one of the men pull a knife from his back pocket and you gasp as you see him plunge this knife into the abdomen of the other man. Your friend shouts, “Call 999!” Fearing for your own safety, you make the call, stating the basic facts of what you have just witnessed to the emergency response team. A few moments later you hear sirens; police and paramedics are on the scene. They want to talk to you. You thought that you would have a nice evening out with friends, and now you are a key witness to a crime. Feeling distressed and shocked by what you just saw, you shake hands with a police officer, and prepare to give your statement. How will the police question you to ensure that they get an accurate and complete account of what happened? Have their interviewing techniques been supported by empirical research? In this chapter, we focus on these questions, highlighting several “best-practice” interviewing techniques.

  Although the situation described above is relatively uncommon, thousands of witnesses or victims of crime are interviewed each year. Many of these individuals are considered “vulnerable” victims or witnesses. For example, in England, more than 50,000 children were the subject of a child protection plan between April 2015 and March 2016, meaning they were identified as needing protection from physical, emotional or sexual abuse and neglect (Department for Education, 2016).

  The intellectually disabled and elderly are also at enhanced risk for becoming victims of crimes (see also Chapter 14). It is important to consider how interview techniques may be tailored to accommodate the needs of particular groups of victims or witnesses.

  In this chapter, we begin by reviewing some of the shortcomings and consequences of traditional investigative interviews and discuss the Cognitive Interview (CI), which was designed to interview cooperative, primarily adult, witnesses in light of these shortcomings. Next, we discuss research on interviewing vulnerable victims and witnesses, focusing on children, the elderly, and individuals with intellectual or developmental disabilities. We discuss the social and cognitive abilities of each, along with research that has examined the most effective ways to elicit accurate and complete recall from these populations. In so doing, we review the Memorandum of Good Practice (MOGP) and Achieving Best Evidence (ABE) guidelines, as well as the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) Investigative Interview Protocol. At times, we use the terms “victims” and “witnesses” interchangeably.

 

‹ Prev