Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing: Current Developments and Future Directions

Home > Other > Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing: Current Developments and Future Directions > Page 16
Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing: Current Developments and Future Directions Page 16

by Ray Bull, Tim Valentine, Dr Tom Williamson


  were more likely to be convicted than those who chose to deny the offence

  during the interview. In general terms, something like three - quarters of all

  suspects answered police questions, with the remainder failing to answer any

  question or being selective in relation to what they have to say. Such fi ndings

  effectively extinguished one of the most enduring of police interviewing myths;

  that all suspects say nothing on the advice of their lawyer.

  More recently, legislative changes introduced under sections 34

  – 37 of

  the Criminal Justice and Public Order Act 1994 allow a judge and/or jury

  to draw adverse inferences from a suspect ’ s silence during police question-

  ing. Initial research concerning this additional power is in many respects

  equivocal. For example, nearly 40% of suspects exercising their RoS were

  given this ‘ special warning ’ , yet relatively few gave a satisfactory account in

  response. On the other hand, the percentage of suspects refusing to answer

  some or all police questions fell from 23% to 16% and the proportion that

  gave a complete ‘ no comment ’ response fell from 10% to 6% (Bucke, Street

  & Brown, 2000 ).

  The original intention behind this additional power was twofold: to dis-

  courage a suspect from fabricating a defence later and to encourage a sus-

  pect to disclose a genuine defence as early as possible in the process. More

  recently, the Home Offi ce has been examining allegations that the opportunity

  to draw adverse inferences against a suspect

  ’ s silence has effectively been

  undermined by the growing popularity amongst defence representatives to

  submit a prepared statement that allows their client to continue with a ‘ no

  comment ’ regime (internal correspondence between Home Offi ce, Police,

  CPS and other agencies). In other words, where it is alleged that a suspect

  carried out a crime, the defence lawyer will collaborate with their client to

  create an often short and meaningless statement that simply refutes the

  allegation.

  An important adjunct to the use of adverse inferences lies in the amount of

  pre - interview disclosure material supplied by the police. The Court of Appeal

  has held that if little or nothing of the case against a suspect is disclosed, so

  that a legal adviser cannot properly advise their client, this may be a good

  reason to advise the suspect to remain silent (see R v Argent [1997] ; R v

  Imran & Hussain [1997] ; R v Noble [1997] CLR 346). The onus is, there-

  fore, on the investigating offi cer to disclose suffi cient information to ensure

  that the suspect understands the nature and circumstances of their arrest.

  There is, however, no requirement for the police to present a prima facie case

  74

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  before questioning ( R v Imran & Hussein ; R v Farrell [2004] ; and the House

  of Lords judgment in

  Ward v Police Service of Northern Ireland [2007] ,

  reported in The Times , 22 November 2007).

  The second contextual factor – the strength of the evidence and, impor-

  tantly, the suspect ’ s perception of this state of affairs

  – was a very crucial

  determinant of behaviour. In the Moston, Stephenson & Williamson (1992)

  study when the evidence was weak, confessions occurred in less than 10% of

  cases and denials in 77%. Conversely, when the evidence was strong, the fi gures

  effectively reversed, with 67% confessions and 16% denials. In terms of the

  facilitative factors infl uencing a suspect what was important was when

  the suspect believed that there was no point in denying the offence because

  the police have suffi cient evidence to eventually prove it. Such personal percep-

  tions are exactly what a number of interrogation manuals recommend should

  be targeted and manipulated by interviewing offi cers. Such tactics stimulate a

  number of legal and ethical issues (for a comprehensive review see Gudjonsson,

  2003 ; for detail of the specifi c tactics, see Inbau, Reid, Buckley & Jayne,

  2001 ).

  The third important contextual factor relates to the type and nature of

  tactics employed by the police. This overlaps with the internal facilitative factor

  of external pressure, which accommodates not only persuasive police tactics

  but also concern regarding police behaviour and fear of confi nement. Research

  has identifi ed a clear distinction between the type of tactics employed in

  general, run - of - the - mill cases and more serious cases (Pearse & Gudjonsson,

  1999 ). In the former it would appear that the suspect has already made up his

  or her mind to confess or deny and is able to stick to that position regardless

  of external factors. Indeed, in such circumstances confessions are often made

  despite the tactics used, rather than because of them. In serious cases, such as

  murder, robbery, blackmail and rape, we are more likely to fi nd coercive,

  manipulative and forceful tactics applied with more intensity and often over a

  much longer period of incarceration ( ibid. ; Pearse, 1997 ).

  In 1997 the author examined a unique dataset of 20 very serious criminal

  cases where the suspects (both male and female) had been persuaded, on audio

  tape, to shift their position from denial to admission, despite the onerous

  consequences attached to an admission to such serious offences Pearse, 1997 ).

  However, fi nding examples of someone shifting their position from denial to

  admission during interview proved very diffi cult, as it is a quite rare occurrence.

  A detailed examination of what actually took place to achieve this shift revealed

  that the interviewing offi cers engaged in a combination of coercive and pro-

  longed tactics that concentrated on exploiting many of the contextual factors

  and manipulating the internal factors identifi ed above. Obvious differences

  with minor cases include duration (in minor cases average interview was

  22 mins, range 2 – 109 mins, in serious cases 2 hrs 16 mins, range 24 mins – 12 hrs

  42 mins), as well as the overbearing nature of ‘ clusters of tactics ’ . Eight cases

  (40%) were ruled inadmissible at court although a legal adviser was present in

  fi ve of those eight cases (63%) (Pearse, 1997 ).

  The Investigation of Terrorist Offences in the United Kingdom

  75

  In summary, whilst a number of factors were concerned with the decision -

  making process of suspects in relation to making a confession or denial, the

  main conclusion that can be drawn from the research ‘ is that the most frequent

  and important reason why suspects confess is the strength of their belief in the

  evidence against them ’ (Gudjonsson, 2003 : 153).

  Recent decades have also seen a growth in the availability of research con-

  ducted into the individual characteristics of persons detained by police who

  may be vulnerable to providing misleading information or who may be unable

  to appreciate the signifi cance of their answers or the consequences attached to

  their predicament. The fi rst empirical research in this area was for the 1993

  Royal Commission. A number of relevant miscarriages of justice have been

  identifi ed that suggest that the root c
auses will rarely be found in one place;

  rather it will be a combination of events and issues that together bring about

  such fl awed prosecutions. Thus a suspect ’ s psychological vulnerability (sug-

  gestibility, compliance and learning disability) may not be recognized or acted

  upon, their pre - existing mental illness or depression may not be acknowledged,

  and they could be subjected to lengthy periods of incarceration and extensive

  interviewing sessions by police. Environmental factors may also play a part

  brought about by continued isolation from peers, the alien and debilitating

  impact of detention, and sleep and food deprivation (Gudjonsson, Rutter,

  Clare & Pearse, 1993 ; see also Gudjonsson, 2003 ). In the terrorist arena a

  contemporaneous example has been created by the circumstances to be found

  at the Guantanamo Bay Detention Centre and Abu Ghraib Prison regime in

  Iraq (Pearse, 2006 ).

  Legal p erspective

  Acts of terrorism are perceived as extreme examples of violence, perpetrated

  against often innocent individuals and special measures have to be introduced

  in order to combat this threat. The readiness of Parliament to bring forward

  new legislation supports such a contention. The same may also be said of the

  judiciary in the UK. Some legal authorities are of the view that

  there is a case for saying that terrorism, and legislation for the prevention of

  terrorism, should be regarded in a special way: the interference with a right or

  freedom may be more readily justifi ed in the case of terrorism. (Rowe, 2000 :

  527 – 542)

  Arguments in support of this stance can be found in the powers and duties

  of states to protect their citizens of the (European Convention on Human

  Rights, Article 2

  – safeguarding the right to life), on moral grounds that

  condemn terrorism as an illegitimate form of violent expression, and fi nally

  that such acts are best understood as special forms of crime requiring a par-

  ticular response to overcome the diffi culties faced by the investigation and

  76

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  prosecution process. It has been argued ‘ That there is no greater challenge for

  a democracy than the response it makes to terrorism ’ (Home Offi ce, 2004 ).

  Evidence of legal recognition for the need to treat terrorist issues as special

  cases can be found in current terrorist legislation and legal judgments (Walker,

  2002 , who described TACT as ‘ the most extensive counter - terrorist code in

  western Europe ’ ). For example, in the UK police powers to stop and search

  have been extended by section 44 of TACT. In March 2003, Brooke LJ and

  Mr Justice Maurice Kay said the use of the random stop and search powers

  and any resulting violation of human rights were justifi ed because ‘ a threat

  greater than any that this country in general and its capital city in particular,

  have ever faced except in time of war ’ (2003 EWHC 2545) – a fi nding upheld

  by the House of Lords in March 2006. Terrorist suspects can, under some

  circumstances, be detained without access to a legal adviser for up to 48 hours

  and in other circumstances, a suspect may only consult a solicitor in the sight

  and hearing of a ‘ qualifi ed ’ police offi cer (Schedule 8 (part I) of TACT paras

  8 and 9 respectively).

  The possession of articles for terrorist purposes, under section 57 of TACT,

  proved to be one of the fi ercest debating points in the UK during the passage

  of the Act as it was interpreted as reversing the burden of proof from the

  prosecution to the defence. In giving judgment on this issue in the House of

  Lords, Lord Cooke was of the opinion that terrorism was to be treated as a

  special case and that it was not disproportionate to put burdens on the defen-

  dant in times of terrorism (Rowe, 2000 : 529; see also the reverse burden

  provisions under section 18(2) of TACT). More recently Parliament has

  extended the detention period that suspects can be held without charge from

  14 days to up to 28 days. This increase was granted after a major political

  debate in Parliament, which saw the government

  ’ s original proposal for a

  period of 90 days ’ detention without charge defeated. The government con-

  tinues to lay proposals before the House for additional periods of detention,

  the latest set at 42 days. Additional offences of encouraging and preparing or

  training for terrorism have been introduced (Terrorism Act 2006, sections 1,

  5 and 6 respectively). Such an environment suggests that what takes place in

  the police interview scenario with terrorist suspects may also be at the bound-

  aries of criminal law and practice, especially when compared with typical

  criminal cases.

  In the UK, proposed changes to the terrorism legislation recently brought

  about considerable parliamentary and public debate and some indication of

  the logistics and scale of the tasks facing terrorist investigators was revealed

  by New Scotland Yard (

  http://www.homeoffi ce.gov.uk/Terrorism/

  Terrorismandthelaw , letter from Assistant Commissioner Andy Hayman to the

  Home Secretary, Rt. Hon. Charles Clarke, 6 October 2005). This suggests

  that, unlike criminal cases, there is often a need to arrest terrorist suspects

  prematurely in order to protect the public and save lives. Such precipitous

  action may be based on sensitive intelligence and often before there has been

  The Investigation of Terrorist Offences in the United Kingdom

  77

  time to gather suffi cient evidence against the terrorist suspects that would

  allow the prosecution to consider there was a likelihood of conviction, unlike

  criminal arrests where the whole ethos behind the introduction of PACE was

  to provide police with suffi cient powers to gather evidence prior to arrest and

  so reduce the period of detention.

  Other obstacles encountered by the police included the extensive nature of

  the many enquiries undertaken in terrorist cases (identity fraud, telephone and

  other communication searches, hard drives and software to be interrogated

  – sometimes encrypted – international liaison, fi nancial aspects) and the need

  for exhaustive forensic searches, some taking up to weeks to conclude. These

  were part of a number of the arguments put forward in order to extend the

  detention of terrorist suspects from 14 days to 90 days.

  Unfortunately, there has been very little comprehensive research into ter-

  rorist cases in the UK and the exact dynamics within this particular interview

  scenario are not well known. The author was recently granted access to analyse

  a number of terrorist cases and to conduct face - to - face interviews with a broad

  range of serving CT offi cers which in turn led to the distribution of a ques-

  tionnaire to serving offi cers within the CTC at New Scotland Yard. In this

  chapter, the main hypothesis under examination is that interviews with terrorist

  suspects will be treated by police in the same manner as serious criminal

  offences and that police offi cers will engage in a range of coercive and manipu-

  lative interviewing tactics in order infl uence the decision - making process of
/>   the suspect to obtain a confession or an admission.

  Methodological c onsiderations

  This research was divided into three sections: an analysis of 30 CT cases; a

  survey of the views of a range of offi cers from senior investigators and manag-

  ers to detective inspectors, sergeants and constables by undertaking personal

  interviews; and a customized questionnaire distributed randomly to investigat-

  ing offi cers (detective sergeants and detective constables). There are restric-

  tions on the amount of data that can be catered for within the confi nes of one

  chapter and I shall limit this review to important distinctions between criminal

  and terrorist cases, the impact of the

  ‘ special warning procedure

  ’ and the

  nature and type of interview tactics adopted by police.

  In order for cases to qualify for this study the researcher needed to have

  access to the following items in each case:

  • the audio - tapes of the terrorist interview;

  • transcripts of all interviews;

  • a copy of the suspect ’ s custody record;

  • sight of the initial Crown Prosecution Service (CPS) report or outline

  details of the case.

  78

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  The CPS report was necessary in order to achieve some understanding of

  the nature of the allegations in each case, and access to the suspect ’ s individual

  custody record provided an insight into any issues relevant to their detention,

  treatment or welfare. This might include medical observations – the result of

  examinations by doctors, information relating to his or her mental state and

  any condition related to the custodial environment.

  The presence of both the audio - tape and the transcript of the interview

  provided the author with a contemporaneous insight into what actually took

  place, as well as an accurate reference document to annotate. This detailed

  methodology is far more extensive than many previous studies which relied

  solely on observational data or only one medium (e.g. transcripts) (see Pearse,

  1997 ). In previous studies access to the audio tapes has proved invaluable in

  correcting a large number of sometimes very serious errors in the court tran-

  scripts – for example, a rape case with the person denying the offence: the

 

‹ Prev