Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing: Current Developments and Future Directions

Home > Other > Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing: Current Developments and Future Directions > Page 45
Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing: Current Developments and Future Directions Page 45

by Ray Bull, Tim Valentine, Dr Tom Williamson


  line - ups or photo arrays produce substantial differences in identifi cation per-

  formance ’ ( 1994 : 181).

  232

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  Fairness of v ideo l ine - u ps

  The research available suggests that video identifi cation is likely to be no more

  or less sensitive than a live line - up. However, the number of video clips avail-

  able in a database to construct video line - ups is much greater than the number

  of volunteers available to stand on a live line - up. Therefore, there is good

  reason to believe that in an operational context it should be possible to con-

  struct video line - ups that are fairer than live line - ups. Indeed, research has

  found that video line - ups from criminal cases were fairer to the suspects than

  conventional ‘ live ’ line - ups (Valentine & Heaton, 1999 ). Furthermore, video

  line - ups were equally fair to white European and African – Caribbean suspects

  (Valentine, Harris, Colom Piera & Darling, 2003 ). In these studies, partici-

  pants ( ‘ mock witnesses ’ ) were given the fi rst description of the offender pro-

  vided by the original witness and were required to select the line - up member

  whom they thought was most likely to be the suspect. As a mock witness has

  not seen the perpetrator, the suspect should be chosen no more often than

  predicted by chance if the line - up is perfectly fair: 11% (one in nine) of choices

  should be of the suspect from line - ups containing eight foils. Valentine &

  Heaton (1999) found that the mock witnesses identifi ed the suspect in live

  line - ups more frequently (25%) than by chance, but were not able to select

  the suspect from video line - ups (15%) signifi cantly more often than chance.

  Valentine et al. (2003) found that video line - ups of African – Caribbeans and

  of white Europeans were equally fair, using equal numbers of mock witness

  from both ethnic backgrounds.

  Perception of i dentifi cation o ffi cers

  Hughes (2005) examined the opinions of 30 experienced police identifi cation

  offi cers of video identifi cation procedures. Almost all identifi cation offi cers

  regarded video identifi cation as a better method than a live line - up. The point

  was made that it facilitated use of identifi cation evidence for volume crime

  rather than just serious crime. Interestingly, 37% of identifi cation offi cers

  thought identifi cation evidence was wholly reliable. This fi gure increases to 51%

  if witness confi dence is high and the procedure is held shortly after the crime.

  The majority of offi cers (66%) thought that video identifi cation is fair to both

  suspect and witness; nobody thought it unfair to the suspect, but 33% thought

  it unfair to the witness. The concern was that aspects of the procedure made

  the task of identifi cation too diffi cult. There was a clear view (90%) that more

  should be done to brief and support the witness prior to the procedure.

  Evaluation of r evised p rocedures for v ideo l ine - u ps

  Valentine, Darling

  & Memon

  (2007) tested whether adopting the strict

  sequential presentation rules described above would enhance the reliability of

  video identifi cation evidence. The strict sequential presentation procedure, by

  Recent Developments in Eyewitness Identifi cation Procedures

  233

  which the witness was instructed to make a response to each face as it was

  presented, was compared to the British procedure in which witnesses are told

  to watch the whole video twice before making a decision. The line - up admin-

  istrator was blind to the position of the suspect in a nine - person line - up under

  both the ‘ strict sequential ’ and the ‘ existing ’ conditions. There was no reliable

  effect of the presentation procedure on the number of mistaken identifi cations

  from perpetrator - absent line - ups, but there were fewer correct identifi cations

  of the perpetrator when he was present in the line - up under the ‘ strict sequen-

  tial ’ condition.

  Darling, Valentine & Memon (2008) tested the use of a culprit - description

  strategy instead of suspect - resemblance strategy to select foils for a nine - person

  video line - up. It was established, from pairwise ratings of similarity provided

  by participants who did not take part in the main study, that the foils in the

  culprit - description line - ups were more dissimilar to each other than were the

  foils in the suspect resemblance line - ups. Nevertheless, there was no statistically

  signifi cant difference in the rate of correct or mistaken identifi cation between

  culprit - description and suspect - resemblance line - ups. The experiments reported

  by Valentine et al. (2007) and Darling et al. (2008) followed British police

  video identifi cation procedures and used line - ups constructed from a police

  national database. These data explicitly compared procedures advocated in the

  research literature with existing procedures in an operational context in the

  UK. There was no evidence that either the sequential presentation instructions

  or the culprit - description strategy would improve the existing video identifi ca-

  tion procedures.

  Suspects with d istinguishing m arks

  How can a fair line

  - up be constructed if a suspect has a distinguishing

  mark, for example a tattoo, scar or distinctive mark on their face? A witness may

  have described a mark or tattoo and the suspect may have been arrested

  because they have a similar mark. Alternatively, the suspect may have a mark

  that was not described by a witness. If the suspect is the only person in the line -

  up with a scar or tattoo on his face, he will stand out in comparison to the foils,

  rendering the line - up unfair. In the UK the suspect ’ s solicitor is likely to object

  to a line - up in which his or her client is the only person with a scar or tattoo.

  There have been two solutions to this problem. Most frequently the area

  of the distinguishing feature is masked on the face of the suspect and the foils.

  For video identifi cation the area is pixelated, using a mosaic of squares in

  a grid that have an average colour and luminance to occlude the area.

  This process can be automated in moving video so that the mosaic occludes

  the same area of the face as the view changes. The alternative strategy is to

  replicate the distinguishing mark on the faces of the foils. This process is time -

  consuming and expensive, so the mark is usually replicated only on full - face

  and profi le views. Under these circumstances still images are shown to the

  witness instead of moving images.

  234

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  Is it better to mask or replicate? The disadvantage of masking a distinguish-

  ing feature is that it changes the appearance of the suspect ’ s face. A distinctive

  feature will be a salient cue to recognizing the face of a perpetrator (Winograd,

  1981 ; Valentine, 1991 ). Therefore, a better strategy is to replicate the feature

  on the foils. If replication is used, the identical feature is normally replicated.

  However, this strategy means that the witness cannot use their knowledge of

  the distinguishing mark to
recognize the perpetrator. A better approach would

  be to use the culprit - description strategy described above. The feature repli-

  cated on each face should be consistent with the witness ’ s description, but the

  precise properties (location, size, colour or style) may vary across foils within

  the constraints set by the description. This variation would allow the witness

  to use recognition of a distinguishing mark to support recognition of the

  perpetrator. However, variation in the characteristics of the distinguishing

  mark would not introduce any bias against an innocent suspect whom the

  witness has not seen before.

  As replication can only be implemented on still images, the question arises

  of whether use of still images rather than moving video would impair identi-

  fi cation accuracy. The effectiveness of moving video and a single, still, full - face

  image was compared in an otherwise identical video identifi cation procedure

  by Valentine et al. (2007) and Darling et al. (2008) . No effect on identifi ca-

  tion of perpetrators from culprit - present line - ups was observed. A foil was less

  likely to be mistakenly identifi ed from culprit - absent line - up when moving

  images were used in one experiment (Valentine et al. , 2007 ), but the effect

  was not replicated in a second experiment (Darling et al. , 2008 ). The data

  suggest that use of moving images has little if any reliable infl uence on the

  outcome of a line - up.

  Eyewitness c onfi dence and a ccuracy

  It has been appreciated for a long time that a confi dent witness may be mis-

  taken (Devlin,

  1976 ). Indeed, a caution to this effect is included in the

  Turnbull judgement on eyewitness evidence. In recent years it has been dem-

  onstrated that the relationship between the confi dence of an eyewitness and

  the accuracy of their identifi cation is moderately strong for witnesses who

  identify somebody at a line - up. However, the relationship is weaker amongst

  witnesses who reject the line

  - up (Sporer, Penrod, Read

  & Cutler,

  1995 ).

  Confi dence is most closely associated with accuracy when measured immedi-

  ately after an identifi cation has been made (Cutler & Penrod, 1989 ), and

  critically before the witness acquires any further information about their iden-

  tifi cation. However, as the association between confi dence and accuracy is far

  from perfect, confi dent but mistaken eyewitnesses will be encountered fairly

  frequently.

  Recent Developments in Eyewitness Identifi cation Procedures

  235

  Malleability of w itness c onfi dence

  A very important research fi nding is that witness confi dence is changeable

  and is infl uenced by information that the witness acquires after attending an

  identifi cation procedure. Receiving feedback that the person identifi ed is the

  suspect, or that somebody else made the same identifi cation, will increase the

  witness ’ s confi dence in their identifi cation. Not only does confi rming feedback

  tend to make the witness subsequently more confi dent in their identifi cation,

  but it also tends to infl ate estimates of a range of subsequent testimony, includ-

  ing how long the culprit was seen for, how close they were and how much

  attention the witness paid (Wells & Bradfi eld, 1998 ). Furthermore, confi rming

  post - identifi cation feedback tends to make eyewitnesses overconfi dent; that is,

  they now express more confi dence in their identifi cation than is warranted

  (Semmler, Brewer & Wells, 2004 ). By the time a witness gives evidence in

  court they are likely to have received confi rming feedback. Therefore, the

  confi dence a witness displays in court may well be determined by the feedback

  they have received in addition to their initial confi dence at the identity

  procedure.

  The c onfi dence of r eal w itnesses

  Wright & Skagerberg (2007) took advantage of the practice of Sussex Police

  to routinely inform witnesses at a video line - up whether they had identifi ed

  the suspect. After the witness had made an identifi cation but before they

  received feedback each witness provided a rating on a 10 - point scale for three

  questions, one each about their opportunity to view the culprit, the identifi ca-

  tion they had just made and how good they believed their general memory to

  be. After receiving feedback, each witness provided a rating on three questions,

  and again one question addressed each of the same three aspects. Witnesses

  evaluated the identifi cation task as more diffi cult after feedback that their

  identifi cation was mistaken, but witnesses who were told that they had identi-

  fi ed the suspect evaluated the task as easier. Wright & Skegerberg ( ibid .) make

  an important contribution by demonstrating that malleability of confi dence

  previously observed in laboratory studies is also observed in real witnesses and

  victims of crime. There is no provision in the current PACE code of practice

  to record a statement of confi dence, although anything the witness does say

  must be written down. In contrast, the US Department of Justice guide on

  eyewitness identifi cation does recommend taking a clear statement of confi -

  dence immediately after the witness makes an identifi cation and before any

  feedback is given (Technical Working Group for Eyewitness Identifi cation,

  1999 ).

  The code of practice for England and Wales requires that witnesses are

  instructed: ‘ If you cannot make a positive identifi cation, you should say so. ’

  Hughes ’ (2005) survey showed that the meaning of the word ‘ positive ’ was

  236

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  ambiguous even amongst identifi cation offi cers. Interpretation ranged from

  ‘ any identifi cation ’ to ‘ a defi nite 100% ’ confi dence. Only 7% of identifi cation

  offi cers said that they ask the eyewitness about their confi dence in their iden-

  tifi cation before they leave the ID suite. Ninety - three per cent of offi cers know

  the position of the suspect in the video line - up (i.e., the identifi cation proce-

  dure is not run blind). The majority (70%) did not think that blind testing

  would enhance justice. Nineteen per cent of police forces have a policy of

  telling witnesses whether they identifi ed the suspect. One reason given for this

  policy is to enhance witness care as most witnesses want to know if they have

  identifi ed the suspect. Unfortunately, only 36% of respondents who inform

  the witness of the outcome of an identifi cation procedure take a statement

  before providing the feedback. Making a written record of confi dence before

  providing feedback is essential to preserve an accurate record of confi dence at

  the time of identifi cation. Prior to trial a witness is likely to receive or deduce

  feedback that will bolster their confi dence.

  Conclusions and r ecommendations

  The evidence is very clear that mistaken identifi cation is the leading cause of

  wrongful conviction. Furthermore, wrongful conviction is a major problem

  – there have now been more than 230 DNA exonerations in the USA. Whilst

  this evidence relates specifi cally to that country, there is good cause to
believe

  that a similar problem exists in the UK. Approximately 20% of eyewitnesses in

  the UK make a known mistaken identifi cation. However, a mistaken identifi ca-

  tion of a foil in a line - up will not lead to a wrongful conviction. It is the

  unknown mistaken identifi cation of the police suspect that leads to a miscar-

  riage of justice. The legal system in the UK is very different from that in the

  USA. Identifi cation procedures in England and Wales are regulated by the

  code of practice (code D) required by the Police and Criminal Evidence Act.

  In addition, a special warning about the frailty of eyewitness identifi cation must

  be given to the jury by the trial judge. Nevertheless, uncorroborated eyewit-

  ness identifi cation evidence remains suffi cient to secure a conviction in England

  and Wales.

  The PACE code of practice does include a number of elements of best

  practice which are not often discussed in the research literature on eyewitness

  identifi cation. The suspect ’ s legal representative has the right to be present

  when a witness views a video or live line - up. If no representative is present,

  the procedure must be videotaped. This provision has the potential to be an

  effective protection of the suspect ’ s rights and to discourage overtly leading

  behaviour. A further provision that allows the suspect or their legal representa-

  tive the opportunity to object to line - up members provides a practical means

  to improve the selection of foils, which is likely to enhance the fairness of

  line - ups.

  Recent Developments in Eyewitness Identifi cation Procedures

  237

  Although the PACE code of practice is extensive, there are a number of

  areas in which practice could be improved. As virtually all identifi cation pro-

  cedures are run on video double - blind testing, in which the line - up administra-

  tor does not know who the suspect is, can be easily implemented (see Valentine,

  2006 , for a practical suggestion of implementing this provision). Blind testing

  would enhance the integrity of identifi cation procedures and safeguard the

  police against accusations of malpractice. The code of practice should be

  amended to include provision to take a clear statement of confi dence imme-

 

‹ Prev