Not Born Yesterday

Home > Other > Not Born Yesterday > Page 17
Not Born Yesterday Page 17

by Hugo Mercier


  from the regime who appeared to accept its values.19 Even now,

  132 ch ap t er 9

  Rus sian propaganda efforts— for example, in Ukraine— follow

  a familiar pattern: succeeding modestly when preaching to the

  choir, backfiring when targeting opponents.20

  Propaganda by the other great communist power, China, was

  barely more convincing, even under Mao. Po liti cal scientist Shao-

  guang Wang studied in detail what motivated the vari ous actors

  of the Cultural Revolution in Wuhan, a large city of central

  China.21 Instead of reflecting a “blind faith in Mao,” citizens’ en-

  gagement with the Cultural Revolution was “a product of

  [their] perception that Mao’s initiative would provide solutions

  to [their] personal prob lems.”22 Those who stood to benefit from

  taking up the cause did so; many others resisted.

  Work on more recent propaganda attempts by the Chinese

  government confirms its broad in effec tive ness. A study of Chi-

  nese citizens’ attitudes toward the government in the mid-1990s

  revealed that consumption of the state- controlled news media

  correlated with lack of trust in the government, making it very

  unlikely that the media successfully instil s trust in the leader-

  ship.23 Lack of trust in official media means that Chinese citizens

  are “always eager to get other information from diff er ent chan-

  nels,” as one of them put it.24 Shortly after Weibo, the Chinese

  equivalent of Twitter, started up, 70 percent of the Chinese who

  used social media admitted relying on them as their primary

  source of information.25 Mistrust of official media and increased

  reliance on other sources mean that rumors presenting a nega-

  tive view of the government are quickly taken up and prove dif-

  ficult to fight.26 That Chinese citizens do not passively accept

  government propaganda is also shown by their many acts of pro-

  test. Journalist Evan Osnos, whose work I have drawn on here,

  reports that, on average, in 2010 there were nearly five hundred

  “strikes, riots, and other ‘mass incidents’ ” taking place across

  China every day— and that is according to official statistics.27

  p r o p a g a nd is t s, c a mp a i g ne r s, a d v e r t ise r s 133

  Having had time to learn the limits of propaganda, the Chi-

  nese Communist Party has shifted strategies for control ing the

  public, away from brute persuasion toward what po liti cal scien-

  tist Margaret Roberts calls “friction and flooding.”28 Friction

  consists in making sensitive information more difficult to

  access— blocking keywords, forcing people to use VPNs, or sim-

  ply not collecting such information in the first place (e.g., infor-

  mation on how wel , or poorly, such and such government agency

  is performing, information that only the state could reliably

  gather). Flooding consists in distracting people from sensitive

  issues by bombarding them with official propaganda. The gov-

  ernment is suspected of having recruited as many as two mil ion

  people to spread messages online— known as the 50 Cent Party,

  after the sum these shil s receive per post. Yet, the government

  seems to have essentially given up on using these legions of pro-

  pagandists to change people’s minds: they “avoid arguing with

  skeptics . . . and [do] not even discuss controversial issues.”29 In-

  stead, they try to either bolster the views of citizens supportive

  of the regime in the first place (a significant number, as we will

  see presently) or talk about other topics, such as celebrity gos-

  sip, distracting the attention of citizens who do not care all that

  much about politics.

  In his book on nondemo cratic regimes, po liti cal scientist

  Xavier Márquez notes several other failures of propaganda:

  “Nearly 40 years of Francoist propaganda did not turn Spaniards

  against democracy . . . constant exposure to the cult of Ceaușescu

  did not turn most Romanians into his partisans . . . unrelenting

  propaganda turned many East Germans into habitual cynics who

  did not believe anything the regime said.”30

  By and large, government propaganda fails to convince the

  public. It can even backfire, leading to widespread distrust of the

  regime. At most, propaganda surfs on preexisting opinions and

  134 ch ap t er 9

  frees people to express what might other wise be seen as socially

  objectionable views.31

  Why, then, do some people in authoritarian regimes behave as

  if they had been brainwashed, saluting the führer in unison, buy-

  ing bil ions of Chairman Mao badges, wailing at Kim Jong- il’s fu-

  neral? The answer is simple. Every authoritarian regime that relies

  on propaganda also closely monitors and violently represses signs

  of dissent. Failure to perform the Nazi salute was perceived as a

  symbol of “po liti cal non- conformism,” a potential death sen-

  tence.32 In North Korea, any sign of discontent can send one’s

  entire family to prison camps.33 Under such threats, we cannot

  expect people to express their true feelings. Describing his life dur-

  ing the Cultural Revolution, a Chinese doctor remembers how “to

  survive in China you must reveal nothing to others.”34 Similarly, a

  North Korean coal miner acknowledged, “I know that our regime

  is to blame for our situation. My neighbor knows our regime is to

  blame. But we’re not stupid enough to talk about it.”35

  When it comes to genuine support, rather than empty dis-

  plays, it seems carrots work better than sticks. Chinese citizens

  might not widely trust the state media, but, on the whole, they

  re spect and support the central government and the Chinese

  Communist Party— which typically garners more than

  70 percent approval, higher than any Western government.36 It

  could be propaganda. Or it could be that under the party’s di-

  rection, China has had high growth rates for de cades, lifting eight

  hundred mil ion people out of poverty.37

  Campaigners

  As we have seen repeatedly, forceful attempts at mass persuasion

  by propagandists in authoritarian regimes fail to sway the popu-

  lation. Rather than the public exercising due vigilance, however,

  p r o p a g a nd is t s, c a mp a i g ne r s, a d v e r t ise r s 135

  could it be that these failures reflect instead the propagandists’

  lack of sophistication or skil ? Goebbels, for example, didn’t seem

  to have been much of a master influencer: by 1940 already, citi-

  zens had lost any interest in official propaganda, on account of

  its “boring uniformity” (as reported by the Nazis’ intel igence

  ser vice).38

  The campaign man ag ers, spin doctors, marketers, pol sters,

  crisis con sul tants, and other specialists who proliferate in con-

  temporary democracies may be more astute. Authoritarian pro-

  paganda relies on monopolistic control of the media: Maybe the

  lack of competition has blunted the instincts and motivations of

  the chief propagandists? By contrast, modern po liti cal campaigns

  are fiercely fought, providing p
lenty of opportunities for profes-

  sionals to refine their skil s and learn how to guide a candidate

  to victory, as well as for candidates to figure out who can best help

  them get elected.

  I focus here on U.S. politics, for two reasons. First, U.S. politi-

  cians vastly outspend other politicians: in 2016, $6.4 bil ion was

  spent on po liti cal campaigns (a third on the presidential race).39

  Second, that’s where the vast majority of studies are conducted.

  If the amounts involved are extraordinary, U.S. electoral cam-

  paigns—in par tic u lar the most high- profile races— are other-

  wise similar to campaigns elsewhere in being presented by the

  press as dramatic events, full of plot twists, with candidates going

  up and down in the pol s as a function of devastating ads, mov-

  ing speeches, and per for mance in public debates. Indeed, given

  the means available— the army of volunteers canvasing door to

  door, the hours of TV ads, the countless robocalls—we would

  expect commensurately dramatic results.

  Yet research on whether po liti cal campaigns and the media

  can win elections, or sway public opinion more generally, has

  given surprisingly ambiguous results. In the first de cades of the

  136 ch ap t er 9

  twentieth century, a popu lar model was that of the “hypodermic

  needle” or the “magic bullet,” according to which people would

  pretty much accept what ever the media were tel ing them.40 This

  model was based on the innovative (but likely in effec tive) use

  of propaganda in World War I, and on a view of the public as re-

  acting reflexively to just about any stimulus they encountered.41

  One thing this model was not based on, though, was data. As

  opinion pol s, tracking of voting be hav ior, and proper studies of

  media influence arose, in the 1940s and 1950s, the era of “mini-

  mal effects” began.42 Summing up years of research, Joseph Klap-

  per stated in 1960 that po liti cal communication “functions

  more frequently as an agent of reinforcement than as an agent

  of change” (a conclusion reminiscent of what the research on

  propaganda shows).43

  The 1970s and 1980s saw the rise of experimental studies in

  po liti cal science. Instead of mea sur ing people’s opinions in the

  field, researchers would bring participants into the lab, expose

  them to vari ous stimuli— campaign materials, TV news, and so

  forth— and mea sure the influence of these stimuli on the par-

  ticipants’ opinions. These techniques revealed that the media had

  the potential to influence public opinion: not by tel ing people

  what to think but by tel ing people what to think about (agenda

  setting), how to best understand issues (framing), and what crite-

  ria to use when evaluating politicians (priming).44 Although these

  effects are less direct than those suggested by the hypodermic-

  needle model, they could still be power ful: people who evaluate

  politicians according to their economic policies rather than

  their views on abortion (say) are likely to vote differently.

  The advantage of these lab- based techniques is the rigor

  of their methods, as they allow researchers to perform well-

  controlled experiments, with participants randomly exposed

  to diff er ent stimuli, their reactions carefully monitored. The

  p r o p a g a nd is t s, c a mp a i g ne r s, a d v e r t ise r s 137

  drawback of these methods is their lack of so- called ecological

  validity: it is hard to tell whether the phenomena observed in

  the lab happen in the uncontrolled environment of real life. For

  instance, some studies showed that exposure to diff er ent pieces

  of news on TV could lead to changes in po liti cal opinions. In

  real life, however, people aren’t passively exposed to TV news:

  they choose which news to watch, or even whether to watch the

  news at all. Po liti cal scientists Kevin Arceneaux and Martin

  Johnson conducted a series of studies in which participants had

  greater leeway in which channel to watch. They observed not

  only that many people simply tuned out but that those people

  who chose to watch the news were those with the most po liti cal

  knowledge, people who were also less likely to change their

  minds in reaction to what they saw on the news.45 Still, even

  looking at more ecologically valid studies, it is clear that po liti cal

  campaigns and the media can shape public opinion on some issues.

  But the way they do so reveals that people do not unquestion-

  ingly accept what ever message po liti cal campaigns put forward.

  By far the most impor tant moderator of whether campaigns

  or the media influence public opinion is the strength of people’s

  prior opinions. On the vast majority of po liti cal issues, people

  have no strong opinion, or even no opinion whatsoever— which

  makes sense, given the time and effort required to garner infor-

  mation on any topic. For example, in the run-up to the 2000 U.S.

  presidential election, few voters were aware of what position

  George W. Bush and Al Gore (the two main candidates) held on

  Social Security.46 As a result, when people were told that the can-

  didate from the party they favored had such and such opinion,

  they tended to adopt this opinion, following “party cues.”47 Fol-

  lowing party cues reflects the (largely) sound working of trust

  mechanisms: if you have come to trust a party over many years,

  it makes sense to follow its lead on issues about which you have

  138 ch ap t er 9

  little knowledge. Citizens are also quite skilled at recognizing

  who among them is most knowledgeable on po liti cal issues, and

  at taking their opinion into account.48 On the whole, people are

  more influenced by reliable signals. For example, a newspaper

  sends a more reliable signal when it endorses a surprising

  candidate— one who doesn’t belong to the party usually sup-

  ported by the newspaper— and people are only influenced (if

  at all) by these surprising endorsements.49

  In the first de cade of the twenty- first century, po liti cal scien-

  tists began conducting large- scale experiments on the effective-

  ness of po liti cal campaigns, sending flyers to a random subset of

  counties, canvassing a random subset of houses, calling a random

  subset of potential voters, and so forth. Opinion surveys or vot-

  ing outcomes were then recorded, allowing the researchers to

  precisely estimate the effects of their intervention— the letter,

  the face- to- face discussion, the call—on participants who had

  been exposed to it, compared with other wise similar participants

  who hadn’t. This methodology offered the best of both worlds:

  it was rigorous yet ecologically valid.

  In 2018, po liti cal scientists Joshua Kalla and David Broockman

  published a meta- analysis of all the studies that respected these

  rigorous methods, to which they added some new data of their

  own.50 Some of the campaign efforts carried out a long time be-

  fore the election had a small but significant effect on voting

  inte
ntions. Early on in the election cycle, people have had less

  time to develop fixed ideas about who they are going to vote for,

  making their opinions slightly more labile. However, these effects

  were never long- lasting and had all but dis appeared by Election

  Day, so that the campaign efforts had no net effect on voting be-

  hav ior.51 Other studies have shown some effects of campaign

  efforts on elections for which voters have few preconceived ideas,

  p r o p a g a nd is t s, c a mp a i g ne r s, a d v e r t ise r s 139

  as they can’t rely on candidates’ affiliations, such as primaries or

  ballot mea sures.52

  When it comes to the big prize— voting on congressional or

  presidential elections— the overall effect of the campaign efforts

  studied was nil.53 This is quite a remarkable result. In spite of the

  huge sums sunk into mailing, canvassing, calling, and advertis-

  ing, campaign interventions in the most salient elections (in the

  United States at least) seem, as a rule, to have no effect.

  The most recent, sophisticated- looking techniques have done

  nothing to challenge this conclusion. Many of you will have

  heard of Cambridge Analytica, the infamous firm that harvested

  data from Facebook users (often without their consent), created

  psychological profiles of these users, and offered po liti cal cam-

  paigns ads targeted specifically to these psychological profiles.

  According to the Guardian, Cambridge Analytica allowed “de-

  mocracy [to be] highjacked.”54

  In fact, it was a scam.

  Targeted advertising can, it seems, have some limited effects,

  but these have only been proven on product purchases, with rel-

  evant data on the users’ profiles, and the effects were tiny, add-

  ing a few dozen purchases after mil ions of people had seen the

  ads.55 Cambridge Analytica was attempting to influence presi-

  dential elections (something no ad has been shown to do) with

  dubious data on users. Even if the influence of Cambridge Ana-

  lytica’s campaign had been as large as that recorded in experi-

  ments on beauty products, it would only have swayed a few thou-

  sand voters. In real ity, its influence was likely nil. Republican

  po liti cal analysts remember the Cambridge Analytica employ-

  ees “throwing jargon around,” but they never saw “any evidence

  it worked”— unsurprisingly as it was based, still in their words,

 

‹ Prev