The Enigma of Reason: A New Theory of Human Understanding

Home > Other > The Enigma of Reason: A New Theory of Human Understanding > Page 42
The Enigma of Reason: A New Theory of Human Understanding Page 42

by Dan Sperber

Jacob, P. 1997. What minds can do: Intentionality in a non-intentional world. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  Janis, I. L. 1982. Groupthink, 2nd rev. ed. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

  Jefferson, T. 1829. Memoir, correspondence, and miscellanies, from the papers of T. Jefferson, vol. 1. Ed. Thomas Jefferson Randolph. Charlottesville, VA: F. Carr, and Co.

  Jessen, C. 2001. Temperature regulation in humans and other mammals, 2nd ed. New York: Springer.

  Johnson, D. W., and R. T. Johnson. 2009. Energizing learning: The instructional power of conflict. Educational Researcher, 38(1): 37–51.

  Johnson-Laird, P. N. 2006. How we reason. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  Johnson-Laird, P. N., and R. M. J. Byrne. 1991. Deduction. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  ———. 2002. Conditionals: A theory of meaning, pragmatics, and inference. Psychological Review, 109: 646–678.

  Kaczynski, T. 2005. The Unabomber manifesto: Industrial society and its future. Berkeley, CA: Jolly Roger Press.

  Kahneman, D. 2003a. Maps of bounded rationality: Psychology for behavioral economics. American Economic Review, 93(5): 1449–1475.

  ———. 2003b. A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58(9): 697–720.

  ———. 2011. Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus & Giroux.

  Kant, I. 1781/1998. Critique of pure reason. Trans. and ed. Paul Guyer and Allen W. Wood. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  Kanwisher, N., J. McDermott, and M. M. Chun. 1997. The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 17(11): 4302–4311.

  Kay, J. 2011. Among the Truthers: A journey through America’s growing conspiracist underground. New York: Harper Collins.

  Keil, F. C. 1992. The origins of an autonomous biology. In M. R. Gunnar and M. Maratsos, eds., Modularity and constraints in language and cognition: The Minnesota symposia on child psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 103–137.

  Keren, G., and Y. Schul. 2009. Two is not always better than one: A critical evaluation of two-system theories. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4(6): 533–550.

  Kesson, E. M., et al. 2012. Effects of multidisciplinary team working on breast cancer survival: Retrospective, comparative, interventional cohort study of 13 722 women. British Medical Journal, 344: e2718.

  Keynes, J. M. 1936. The general theory of employment, interest and money. London: Macmillan.

  ———. 1989. Collected writings, vol. 12. London: Macmillan.

  Khemlani, S., and P. N. Johnson-Laird. 2012. Theories of the syllogism: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 138(3): 427–457.

  ———. 2013. The processes of inference. Argument & Computation, 4(1): 4–20.

  King, P. 1991. Mediaeval thought-experiments: The metamethodology of mediaeval science. In T. Horowitz and G. J. Massey, eds., Thought experiments in science and philosophy. Savage, MD: Rowman & Littlefield, pp. 43–64.

  Kitcher, P. 1993. The advancement of science: Science without legend, objectivity without illusions. New York: Oxford University Press.

  Knobe, J. 2010. Person as scientist, person as moralist. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(4): 315–329.

  Koenig, M. A. 2012. Beyond semantic accuracy: Preschoolers evaluate a speaker’s reasons. Child Development, 83(3): 1051–1063.

  Köhler, W. 1930. Gestalt psychology. London: Bell.

  Koriat, A. 2012. When are two heads better than one and why? Science, 336(6079): 360–362.

  Koriat, A., S. Lichtenstein, and B. Fischhoff. 1980. Reasons for confidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory and Cognition, 6: 107–118.

  Kornblith, H. 2012. On reflection. New York: Oxford University Press.

  Kovács, Á. M. 2016. Belief files in theory of mind reasoning. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 7(2): 509–527.

  Kovács, Á. M., E. Téglás, and A. D. Endress. 2010. The social sense: Susceptibility to others’ beliefs in human infants and adults. Science, 330(6012): 1830–1834.

  Kravitz, D. A., and B. Martin. 1986. Ringelmann rediscovered: The original article. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(5): 936–941.

  Kruglanski, A. W., and G. Gigerenzer. 2011. Intuitive and deliberate judgments are based on common principles. Psychological Review, 118(1): 97–109.

  Kruglanski, A. W., et al. 2006. On parametric continuities in the world of binary either ors. Psychological Inquiry, 17(3): 153–165.

  Kugler, T., E. E. Kausel, and M. G. Kocher. 2012. Are groups more rational than individuals? A review of interactive decision making in groups. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 3(4): 471–482.

  Kuhn, D. 1991. The skills of arguments. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  Kuhn, D., and A. Crowell. 2011. Dialogic argumentation as a vehicle for developing young adolescents’ thinking. Psychological Science, 22(4): 545–552.

  Kuhn, D., V. Shaw, and M. Felton. 1997. Effects of dyadic interaction on argumentative reasoning. Cognition and Instruction, 15(3): 287–315.

  Kuhn, T. 1962. The structure of scientific revolutions, 50th anniv. ed. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

  Kunda, Z. 1990. The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108: 480–498.

  Landemore, H. 2013. Democratic reason: The mechanisms of collective intelligence in politics. In J. Elster and H. Landemore, eds., Collective wisdom. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 251–289.

  Latané, B., and J. Rodin. 1969. A lady in distress: Inhibiting effects of friends and strangers on bystander intervention. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 5(2): 189–202.

  Latané, B., K. Williams, and S. Harkins. 1979. Many hands make light the work: The causes and consequences of social loafing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(6): 822–832.

  Laughlin, P. R. 2011. Group problem solving. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

  Leman, P. J., and G. Duveen. 1999. Representations of authority and children’s moral reasoning. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29(5–6): 557–575.

  Leslie, A. M. 1987. Pretense and representation: The origins of a “theory of mind.” Psychological Review, 94(4): 412–426.

  Levin, I., and S. Druyan. 1993. When sociocognitive transaction among peers fails: The case of misconceptions in science. Child Development, 64(5): 1571–1591.

  Levin, S. G., P. E. Stephan, and M. B. Walker. 1995. Planck’s principle revisited: A note. Social Studies of Science, 25(2): 275–283.

  Levinson, S. C. 2005. Living with Manny’s dangerous idea. Discourse Studies, 7(4–5): 431–453.

  ———. 2006. On the human “interaction engine.” In N. J. Enfield and S. C. Levinson, eds., Roots of human sociality. Oxford: Berg, pp. 39–69.

  Lévy-Bruhl, L. 1910. How natives think. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

  Liberman, V., et al. 2012. Naïve realism and capturing the “wisdom of dyads.” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(2): 507–512.

  Lilienfeld, S. O., R. Ammirati, and K. Landfield. 2009. Giving debiasing away: Can psychological research on correcting cognitive errors promote human welfare? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4(4): 390–398.

  Linstone, H. A., and M. Turoff, eds. 1976. The Delphi method: Techniques and applications. New York: Addison-Wesley.

  Liszkowski, U., et al. 2004. Twelve-month-olds point to share attention and interest. Developmental Science, 7(3): 297–307.

  Liu, Y. 1996. Three issues in the argumentative conception of early Chinese discourse. Philosophy East and West, 46(1): 33–58.

  Lloyd, G. E. R. 2007. Towards a taxonomy of controversies and controversiality: Ancient Greece and China. In M. Dascal and H. Chang, eds., Traditions of controversy. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 3–16.

  Long, E. 1774. The history of Jamaica or, A general survey of the antient and modern state of that island: With reflexions on
its situation, settlements, inhabitants, climate, products, commerce, laws, and Government. Montreal: McGill–Queen’s University Press.

  Louis Lee, N. Y., G. P. Goodwin, and P. N. Johnson-Laird. 2008. The psychological puzzle of Sudoku. Thinking & Reasoning, 14(4): 342–364.

  Lucas, E. J., and L. J. Ball. 2005. Think-aloud protocols and the selection task: Evidence for relevance effects and rationalisation processes. Thinking & Reasoning, 11(1): 35–66.

  Luo, Y., L. Kaufman, and R. Baillargeon. 2009. Young infants’ reasoning about physical events involving inert and self-propelled objects. Cognitive Psychology, 58(4): 441–486.

  Luria, A. R. 1934. The second psychological expedition to central Asia. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 41: 255–259.

  ———. 1976. Cognitive development: Its cultural and social foundations. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  Luskin, R. C., et al.. 2014. Deliberating across deep divides. Political Studies, 62(1): 116–135.

  Luther, M. 1536/1960. The disputation concerning man. Luther’s Works, vol. 34. Ed. Helmut T. Lehman and Lewis W. Spitz. Minneapolis: Augsburg Fortress.

  Mahoney, M. J. 1977. Publication prejudices: An experimental study of confirmatory bias in the peer review system. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 1(2): 161–175.

  Mahoney, M. J., and B. G. DeMonbreun. 1977. Psychology of the scientist: An analysis of problem-solving bias. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 1(3): 229–238.

  Malle, B. F. 2004. How the mind explains behavior: Folk explanations, meaning, and social interaction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  Mancosu, P. 1999. Between Vienna and Berlin: The immediate reception of Godel’s incompleteness theorems. History and Philosophy of Logic, 20(1): 33–45.

  Mandela, N. 1994. Long walk to freedom. Boston: Little, Brown.

  Manktelow, K. 2012. Thinking and reasoning: An introduction to the psychology of reason, judgment and decision making, Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

  Manktelow, K., and M. C. Chung, eds. 2004. Psychology of reasoning: Theoretical and historical perspectives. London: Psychology Press.

  Marler, P. 1991. The instinct to learn. In S. Carey and R. Gelman, eds., The epigenesis of mind: Essays on biology and cognition. London: Psychology Press, pp. 591–617.

  Mascaro, O., and O. Morin. 2014. Gullible’s travel: How honest and trustful children become vigilant communicators. In L. Robinson and S. Einav, eds., Trust and skepticism: Children’s selective learning from testimony. London: Psychology Press, pp. 69–82.

  Mascaro, O., and D. Sperber. 2009. The moral, epistemic, and mindreading components of children’s vigilance towards deception. Cognition, 112: 367–380.

  McAfee, R. P., H. M. Mialon, and S. H. Mialon. 2010. Do sunk costs matter? Economic Inquiry, 48(2): 323–336.

  Mellers, B., et al. 2014. Psychological strategies for winning a geopolitical forecasting tournament. Psychological Science, 25(5): 1106–1115.

  Meltzoff, A. N. 1988. Infant imitation after a 1-week delay: Long-term memory for novel acts and multiple stimuli. Developmental Psychology, 24(4): 470–476.

  Mercier, H. 2011a. On the universality of argumentative reasoning. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 11: 85–113.

  ———. 2011b. Reasoning serves argumentation in children. Cognitive Development, 26(3): 177–191.

  ———. 2012. Looking for arguments. Argumentation, 26(3): 305–324.

  ———. 2016a. The argumentative theory: Predictions and empirical evidence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(9): 689–700.

  ———. 2016b. Confirmation (or myside) bias. In R. Pohl, ed., Cognitive illusions. London: Psychology Press, pp. 99–114.

  ———. In press. Reasoning and argumentation. In V. A. Thompson and L. J. Ball, eds., International handbook of thinking & reasoning. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

  Mercier, H., S. Bernard, and F. Clément. 2014. Early sensitivity to arguments: How preschoolers weight circular arguments. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 125: 102–109.

  Mercier, H., M. Deguchi, et al. 2016. The benefits of argumentation are cross-culturally robust: The case of Japan. Thinking & Reasoning, 22(1): 1–15.

  Mercier, H., and H. Landemore. 2012. Reasoning is for arguing: Understanding the successes and failures of deliberation. Political Psychology, 33(2): 243–258.

  Mercier, H., G. Politzer, and D. Sperber. In press. What causes failure to apply the Pigeonhole Principle in simple reasoning problems? Thinking & Reasoning.

  Mercier, H., and D. Sperber. 2009. Intuitive and reflective inferences. In J. S. B. T. Evans and K. Frankish, eds., In two minds. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 149–170.

  ———. 2011. Why do humans reason? Arguments for an argumentative theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34(2): 57–74.

  Mercier, H., E. Trouche, et al. 2015. Experts and laymen grossly underestimate the benefits of argumentation for reasoning. Thinking & Reasoning, 21(3): 341–355.

  Mercier, H., et al. In press. Natural born arguers: Teaching how to make the best of our reasoning abilities. Educational Psychologist.

  Meulders, M. 2010. Helmholtz: From enlightenment to neuroscience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  Miller, M. B., and M. S. Gazzaniga. 1998. Creating false memories for visual scenes. Neuropsychologia, 36(6): 513–520.

  Miller, S. A., and C. A. Brownell. 1975. Peers, persuasion, and Piaget: Dyadic interaction between conservers and nonconservers. Child Development, 46(4): 992–997.

  Millikan, R. G. 1987. Language, thought, and other biological categories: New foundations for realism. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  ———. 1993. White Queen psychology and other essays for Alice. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  ———. 2004. Varieties of meaning: The 2002 Jean Nicod lectures. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  Minson, J. A., V. Liberman, and L. Ross. 2011. Two to tango. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(10): 1325–1338.

  Moertel, C. G., et al. 1985. High-dose vitamin C versus placebo in the treatment of patients with advanced cancer who have had no prior chemotherapy: A randomized double-blind comparison. New England Journal of Medicine, 312(3): 137–141.

  Monaghan, P., et al. 2014. How arbitrary is language? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369(1651): 20130299.

  Montaigne, M. 1870. The essays of Michael, Seigneur de Montaigne: With notes and quotations, and account of the author’s life. London: Alex Murray & Son.

  Montefiore, S. S. 2003. Stalin: The court of the Red Tsar. London: Knopf.

  Morabia, A. 2006. Pierre-Charles-Alexandre Louis and the evaluation of bloodletting. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 99(3): 158–160.

  Moshman, D., and M. Geil. 1998. Collaborative reasoning: Evidence for collective rationality. Thinking and Reasoning, 4(3): 231–248.

  Mullen, B., C. Johnson, and E. Salas. 1991. Productivity loss in brainstorming groups: A meta-analytic integration. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 12(1): 3–23.

  Myers, D. G., and P. J. Bach. 1974. Discussion effects on militarism-pacifism: A test of the group polarization hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30(6): 741–747.

  Myers, D. G., and G. D. Bishop. 1970. Discussion effects on racial attitudes. Science, 169(3947): 778–779.

  Mynatt, C. R., M. E. Doherty, and R. D. Tweney. 1977. Confirmation bias in a simulated research environment: An experimental study of scientific inference. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29(1): 85–95.

  Nagel, T. 1976. Moral luck. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, supplementary volumes, 50: 115–151.

  Nakamura, H. 1964. Ways of thinking of Eastern peoples: India, China, Tibet, Japan. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.

  Needham, A., and R. Baillargeon. 1993. Intuitions about support in 4.5-month-old infants. Cognition, 47(2): 121–148.

  Nemeth, C. J., and M. Ormiston. 2007. Creative idea generation: Harmony versus stimulation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37(3): 524–535.

 
; Nemeth, C. J., et al. 2004. The liberating role of conflict in group creativity: A study in two countries. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34(4): 365–374.

  Nicastro, N. 2008. Circumference: Eratosthenes and the ancient quest to measure the globe. New York: Macmillan.

  Nickerson, R. S. 1998. Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomena in many guises. Review of General Psychology, 2: 175–220.

  ———. 2012. Aspects of rationality: Reflections on what it means to be rational and whether we are. London: Psychology Press.

  Nielsen, M., and K. Tomaselli. 2010. Overimitation in Kalahari Bushman children and the origins of human cultural cognition. Psychological Science, 21(5): 729–736.

  Nisbett, R. E., and L. Ross. 1980. Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

  Nisbett, R. E., and T. Wilson. 1977. Telling more than we can know. Psychological Review, 84(1): 231–259.

  Nisbett, R. E., et al. 1983. The use of statistical heuristics in everyday inductive reasoning. Psychological Review, 90(4): 339–363.

  Oaksford, M., and N. Chater. 1991. Against logicist cognitive science. Mind & Language, 6(1): 1–38.

  ———. 2003. Optimal data selection: Revision, review, and reevaluation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(2): 289–318.

  ———. 2007. Bayesian rationality: The probabilistic approach to human reasoning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  Oaksford, M., and U. Hahn. 2004. A Bayesian approach to the argument from ignorance. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58(2): 75–85.

  Onishi, K. H., and R. Baillargeon. 2005. Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308: 255–258.

  Open Science Collaboration. 2015. Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science, 349(6251), p. aac4716.

  Oreskes, N. 1988. The rejection of continental drift. Historical Studies in the Physical and Biological Sciences, 18(2): 311–348.

  Origgi, G. 2015. La réputation. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

  Origgi, G., and D. Sperber. 2000. Evolution, communication and the proper function of language. In P. Carruthers and A. Chamberlain, eds., Evolution and the human mind: Modularity, language and meta-cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 140–169.

 

‹ Prev