The Book of Memory

Home > Other > The Book of Memory > Page 20
The Book of Memory Page 20

by Mary Carruthers


  few words (the incipit) of each psalm, so that as one visualizes the number

  one, one simultaneously visualizes ‘‘Beatus vir qui non abiit’’; upon seeing

  the number twenty-two one also sees the text ‘‘Dominus regit me’’; and so

  forth. In Hugh’s scheme the images are the written words as they actually

  appear in a manuscript and the locus is simply a numbered box, but the

  incidental difference of this scheme from the architectural one is less

  important than its fundamental psychological similarity; they both employ

  a system of consciously adopted, rigidly ordered backgrounds as a grid

  which is then filled with the images constituted by the text.

  Hugh also counsels that this same method of numerical ordering can be

  used to learn the text of an individual psalm. Under number twenty-two,

  for example, one might visualize a subsidiary set of consecutive numbers.

  102

  The Book of Memory

  To these one attaches the rest of the text, in short pieces (verses) – however

  many one needs to complete the task. The crucial task for recollection is the

  construction of the orderly grid through which one can bring to mind

  specific pieces of text. This enables one ‘‘when asked, without hesitation

  [to] answer, either in forward order, or by skipping one or several, or in

  reverse order and recited backwards’’ whatever is in the memorized text as a

  whole. And it also enables one to construct mentally a concordance of the

  text, thus disputing quickly and surely, making citations to authorial texts

  by number alone.9

  This scheme will work for any book of the Bible (or for any text at all, for

  that matter). A long text must always be broken up into short segments,

  numbered, then memorized a few pieces at a time. We have some clue as to

  just how short ‘‘short’’ was from the length of verses in the medieval format

  of the Psalms, and from the number of words enclosed in cola and commata

  divisions. Obviously, optimal length varies slightly from one individual

  memory to the next, but the medieval texts of the Psalms generally contain

  more verse divisions than do modern texts. Psalm 23 (Vulgate 22), for

  instance, is six verses long in the printed King James Bible, but has nine

  divisions in the Latin Paris text of the thirteenth century. The longest of

  these is the first, containing thirteen words; by contrast the King James

  contains thirty words in verse four and twenty-two in verse five. The fewer

  number of words per division in the medieval format accords with the real

  limits of human working memory, that is, how much one can safely take in

  during a single conspectus, or mental glance (to use the terminology of the

  memorial artes).10

  Hugh’s mnemonic advice is addressed to students, but not to children

  just learning to read. This is evident when we ask ourselves at what point in

  a monastic education such a work would become useful. The linea or

  number grid he describes imagines the psalms in their textual order, from 1

  onward to 150. The heavy emphasis which is placed upon the incipit or first

  phrase of each psalm is also noteworthy. Though, he says, one may make

  subsidiary cells in the grid for each verse of a psalm, its initial words receive

  Hugh’s most careful attention, as the cues and starting-off points for

  recollection of the whole. Yet, as described, this exercise seems at odds

  with what we know of the monks’ usual recitation practices, and thus needs

  some further exploration.

  At St. Victor, as in all monasteries at this time, novices first learned the

  Psalms by heart in the liturgical offices, as they had for centuries, chanting

  the words to melodies, perhaps accompanied as well with processional

  movements and postures. Entire texts became rote-retained through this

  Elementary memory design

  103

  exercise. The liturgical order of psalms is not, however, that of their textual

  order. Rather, the liturgical order groups all the Psalms in weekly cycles,

  assigning them to particular offices in a variety of ways that is quite

  independent of textual order. So if a novice already had all the Psalm

  texts by rote from chanting the liturgical offices, what could be the point of

  learning them again in a different, textual order by means of the grid device

  Hugh describes? Evidently it was not to learn the Psalms by rote – that had

  already been achieved. Those pueri whom Hugh addressed already had

  their rote memory of these texts.

  The goal must be further study, to be able to find a specific text without

  the need to repeat the whole liturgical sequence. And that is what Hugh

  counsels: ‘‘dispose it in such a manner that when your reason asks for it, you

  are easily able to find it.’’ When your reason asks for it refers to the rational,

  investigative procedures of recollection. The device Hugh describes is good

  not for habitual, rote retention (melody, gesture, and physical location are

  superior for that purpose), but for quick recollection and retrieval, for

  creative study and for invention in situations such as preaching, teaching,

  and debate. Hugh’s advice is for more advanced students, not for begin-

  ners. Indeed, the lists of historical markers in the Chronicle which this

  advice prefaces are not matters for beginning readers, but for serious

  students of the Bible.

  The numerical grid provides random, multiple access points to material

  already in memory, enabling someone to work with and make something

  new from their rote memory store. The mnemonic cue, which is the

  incipit of the verse, crucially provides the starting point for recollection of

  each rote-retained textual chunk. And if a text has been retained with care

  and frequently practiced (conditions provided for by the divine office),

  each cue will reliably call forth the whole. 11 The essential task is to impress

  rote memories as a complete experience, made as particular as possible. To

  that end:

  we [should] also pay attention carefully to those circumstances of our matters

  which can occur accidentally and externally, so that, for example, together with the

  appearance and quality or location of the places in which we heard one thing or the

  other, we recall the face and habits of the people from whom we learned this and

  that, and, if there are any, the things that accompany the performance of a certain

  activity. All these things indeed are rudimentary in nature, but of a sort beneficial

  for youths. 12

  Advice to pay particular attention to manuscript layout, where and how a

  text is presented on a page, is commonplace, and I will discuss other

  examples of it shortly. Hugh counsels as well to set our memories within

  104

  The Book of Memory

  the seasons and times, and the performative expessions and gestures that

  occured when one first learned the verses. Such experiential cues are

  perhaps best explained with reference to liturgical performances, for

  which time and season are crucially important. Thus Hugh addresses

  situations of memorizing both from written books and also from physical

  performance. The two modes reinforce one ano
ther, both providing addi-

  tional experiential cues to the rote-retained segments, which help to recover

  them richly within one’s investigative grid.

  Hugh’s advice to divide the text duplicates that of Quintilian, as it also

  articulates the experience of Shereshevsky, who had difficulty remembering

  long passages of connected words, but none at all in retaining an apparently

  limitless number of short segments. Any long text can be treated as though

  it were composed of a number of short ones:

  For the memory always rejoices [says Hugh] in both brevity of length and fewness in

  number, and therefore it is necessary, when the sequence of your reading tends

  toward length, that it first be divided into a few units, so that what the memory

  could not comprehend as a single expanse it can comprehend at least in a number. 13

  This is advice which Hugh repeated in Didascalicon. In this treatise

  Hugh extolls even more the dependency of all wisdom (sapientia) and the

  liberal arts upon memory, training which is now sadly decayed because

  students do not learn proper habits:

  We read that men studied these seven [arts] with such zeal that they had them

  completely in memory, so that whatever writings they subsequently took in hand

  or whatever questions they posed for solution or proof, they did not thumb the

  pages of books to hunt for rules and reasons which the liberal arts might afford for

  the resolution of a difficult matter, but at once had the particulars ready by heart.

  Hence, it is a fact that in that time there were so many learned men that they alone

  wrote more than we are able to read.14

  Hugh’s Preface shows this same contempt for the cumbersome, inefficient,

  and happenstance method of turning the pages of a book to look for a text

  that one needs. Do you think, he asks the students, that people wanting to

  cite a particular Psalm turn over the pages of a manuscript hunting for it?

  ‘‘The labor in such a task would be too great.’’15 It is also striking that Hugh

  makes an exact correlation here between the amount stored in one’s

  memory and the amount of written composition one produces.

  But Hugh’s most practical advice in Didascalicon, a work which teaches

  the arts of reading as its title indicates, is to gather (colligere) while reading,

  ‘‘reducing to a brief and compendious outline things which have been

  written or discussed at some length.’’16 Again the principle of dividing a

  long text (prolixius) is to be observed, because, Hugh says, the memory is

  Elementary memory design

  105

  lazy and rejoices in brevity.17 Therefore, we ought to gather something brief

  and secure from everything we learn, which we can store away in the little

  chest of our memory. 18

  One should not assume that Hugh meant that one should retain only a

  compact summary of what one has read; what he means is that one should

  break prolixity, a long text, into a number of short, securely retained segments

  which can be gathered in the memory. This method of study certainly leads to

  florilegia, as it leads also to Bartlett’s Familiar Quotations and The Oxford

  Dictionary of Quotations. The phrase ‘ brevem . . . et compendiosam sum-

  mam,’’ ‘ a brief and compendious summary,’’ might seem self-contradictory,

  except that Hugh is clearly giving the same advice he spells out more fully in

  his Chronicle Preface, memorizing a compendious summation of brief seg-

  ments of the text one is trying to master – the scholar’s method of note-taking,

  in other words, except written in the memory instead of on note-cards. It is

  worth recalling by those who might dismiss such advice as mere florilegiality,

  born of distaste for the comprehensive knowledge of a text, that note-taking

  and serious scholarship are not exclusive activities. How compendious the

  summation of a text might be would depend on the industry and talent of each

  individual reader, and the importance to him of a particular text.

  This principle of grouping or ‘‘gathering’ respects the limits of working

  memory. It is called ‘ chunking’ in neuropsychology now.19 While the

  storage capacity of memory is virtually limitless, the amount of information

  that can be focused upon and comprehended at one time is definitely

  limited, to a number of units somewhere between five and nine; some

  psychologists express it as a law of ‘ Seven plus-or-minus two.’ 20 So one of

  the fundamental principles for increasing mnemonic (recollective) efficiency

  is to organize single bits of information into informationally richer units by a

  process of substitution that compresses large amounts of material into single

  markers. In this way, while one is still limited by one’s capacity to focus on

  no more than six or seven units at a time, each unit can be made much richer.

  As the psychologist George Miller has written (without being aware that he

  was echoing one of Hugh of St. Victor’s favorite images), if my purse holds

  only six coins I can carry six pennies or six dimes; similarly, it is as easy to

  memorize a list containing a lot of information coded into ‘‘rich’’ units as it is

  to memorize one containing ‘ poor’ units, for the limiting factor is the

  number, not the nature of each item. Miller describes grouping in this way:

  The material is first organized into parts which, once they cohere, can be replaced

  by other symbols – abbreviations, initial letters, schematic images, names, or what

  have you – and eventually the whole scope of the argument is translated into a few

  symbols which can be grasped all at one time.21

  106

  The Book of Memory

  This is exactly what Hugh counsels doing when he substitutes number-

  coordinates for the verses of a Psalm – the active, working memory first

  focuses on the number, and then that numerical address leads to the text

  placed within it, itself composed of a few words at a time, grouped into

  phrases. In recollection, one first focuses on the informationally richest

  sign, say ‘‘Psalm 23.’’ That stands in for a set of six sub-units or verses (in the

  King James Version), and one might focus on one number amongst those,

  perhaps the number two. That sign in turn both stands for and cues the

  words ‘‘He maketh me to lie down in green pastures; he leadeth me beside

  the still waters,’’ themselves grouped into five syntactic sub-units or

  phrases. A fairly common method of citing texts in commentaries and

  other learned material, especially Psalm texts (the most widely known by

  heart), was by the initial letters of each word, so that, for example, ‘‘Beatus

  uir qui non abiit’’ (Ps. 1:1) could be written ‘‘b. u. q. n. a.’’ This is another

  substitution process of the sort Miller adduces, a means of making psycho-

  logically richer units. It saved work for the scribe, but it also served as a

  memory note of effective brevity, for readers who were expected to know

  the words referred to such signs.

  Because of the substitution process that creates rich units, one can skip

  material, rearrange it, collate it, or whatever, simply by manipulating a

  few digits mentally – recalling the second verse of ev
ery Psalm, perhaps, or

  reciting a couple of Psalms by alternating verses from one with the other,

  maybe one in ascending order and the other in the reverse. Any number of

  impressive parlor-tricks (ancient and medieval pedagogy would have

  called them exercises) can be played, for one is actually just counting a

  few digits at a time. One can also use the ability conferred by this process

  for serious ends, such as marshaling texts on a particular topic, as Hugh

  suggests in his Preface, by proferentes numerum, ‘‘fetching forth . . . its

  number,’’22 that is, memorizing only the numerical coordinates to a text

  under the topical key-word. These coordinates then trigger recollection of

  each separate text. It would indeed be possible to conduct a substantive

  discussion of doctrine by numerical citations alone among scholars who

  knew their Bible in this fashion.

  Division and composition

  There is a trope in late medieval university prose called a divisio or a

  discretio: we might call it a summary outline or even a set of study notes.

  Such a ‘‘division’’ comprises the main topics of a particular subject under

  study, and one finds them in the margins of academic manuscripts such as

  Elementary memory design

  107

  the Decretals and the Sentences, works designed for commentary and

  disputation. A fine example is in the fifteenth-century Bolognese manu-

  script of the Decretals (a canon law text) reproduced in figure 4. This

  divisio concerns papal powers, and the subdivisions of the topic spew forth

  from the beak of a memorable imperial eagle, each written on a wavy line

  emanating like speech from the eagle’s mouth. The whole image provides a

  unique, succinct, and effective cue for these subject matters, indicating how

  they are related in content and what their underlying theme is, for these

  powers all relate to the crucial fourteenth-century tension between the

  papacy and the lay monarchs who both sponsored it and fought each other

  through its offices and influence.

  Two late Roman grammarians, Consultus Fortunatianus and Julius

  Victor, define the process and goal of division clearly, and since each was

  quite influential throughout the history of medieval pedagogy, it is worth

 

‹ Prev