The Gap

Home > Other > The Gap > Page 39
The Gap Page 39

by Thomas Suddendorf


  Corballis, M. C. (2007b). The uniqueness of human recursive thinking. American Scientist, 95, 242–250.

  Corballis, M. C. (2011). The recursive mind: The origins of human language, thought, and civilization. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

  Corballis, M. C., & Suddendorf, T. (2010). The evolution of concepts: A timely look. In D. Marshal et al. (Eds.), The making of human concepts (pp. 365–389). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  Corbey, R. (2005). The metaphysics of apes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  Correia, S. P. C., et al. (2007). Western scrub-jays anticipate future needs independently of their current motivational state. Current Biology, 17, 856–861.

  Cosmides, L., et al. (2005). Detecting cheaters. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 505–506.

  Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 87–114.

  Crespi, B., & Badcock, C. (2008). Psychosis and autism as diametrical disorders of the social brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 241–261.

  Cruciani, F., et al. (2011). A revised root for the human Y chromosomal phylogenetic tree: The origin of patrilineal diversity in Africa. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 88, 814–818.

  Csibra, G., et al. (1999). Goal attribution without agency cues: The perception of ‘pure reason’ in infancy. Cognition, 72, 237–267.

  Curnoe, D., et al. (2012). Human remains from the Pleistocene-Holocene transition of Southwest China suggest a complex evolutionary history for East Asians. PLOS One, 7, e31918.

  Custance, D. M., et al. (1995). Can young chimpanzees imitate arbitrary actions? Hayes and Hayes (1952) revisited. Behaviour, 132, 839–858.

  D’Argembeau, A., & Van der Linden, M. (2004). Phenomenal characteristics associated with projecting oneself back into the past and forward into the future: Influence of valence and temporal distance. Consciousness and Cognition, 13, 844–858.

  D’Argembeau, A., & Van der Linden, M. (2008). Remembering pride and shame: Self-enhancement and the phenomenology of autobiographical memory. Memory, 16, 538–547.

  D’Argembeau, A., et al. (2008). Remembering the past and imagining the future in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 247–251.

  d’Errico, F., & Stringer, C. B. (2011). Evolution, revolution or saltation scenario for the emergence of modern cultures? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 366, 1060–1069.

  Dalen, L., et al. (2012). Partial turnover in Neandertals: Continuity in the East and population replacement in the West. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 29, 1893–1897.

  Daly, M., & Wilson, M. A. (1988). Evolutionary social psychology and family homicide. Science, 242, 519–524.

  Dart, R. A. (1925). Australopithecus africanus: The man-ape of South Africa. Nature, 115, 195–199.

  Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex (2003 ed.). London: Gibson Square Books.

  Darwin, C. (1873). The expressions of the emotions in man and animal. London: Murray.

  Darwin, C. (1877). A biographical sketch of an infant. Mind, 2, 285–294.

  Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  Dawkins, R. (2000). An open letter to Prince Charles. Retrieved from http://www.edge.org/3rd_culture/prince/prince_index.html

  Deacon, T. (1997). The symbolic species: The co-evolution of language and the brain. New York: W.W. Norton.

  Dean, L. G., et al. (2012). Identification of the social and cognitive processes underlying human cumulative culture. Science, 335, 1114–1118.

  Deaner, R. O., et al. (2007). Overall brain size, and not encephalization quotient, best predicts cognitive ability across non-human primates. Brain Behavior and Evolution, 70, 115–124.

  Deary, I., et al. (2010). The neuroscience of human intelligence differences. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 201–211.

  deCastro, J. M. B., et al. (1997). A hominid from the lower Pleistocene of Atapuerca, Spain: Possible ancestor to Neanderthals and modern humans. Science, 276, 1392–1395.

  Defleur, A., et al. (1999). Neanderthal cannibalism at Moula-Guercy, Ardeche, France. Science, 286, 128–131.

  Dehaene, S. (2009). Reading in the brain. New York: Penguin Books.

  Del Cul, A., et al. (2009). Causal role of prefrontal cortex in the threshold for access to consciousness. Brain, 132, 2531–2540.

  DeLoache, J. S., & Burns, N. M. (1994). Early understanding of the representational function of pictures. Cognition, 52, 83–110.

  Denault, L. K., & McFarlane, D. A. (1995). Reciprocal altruism between male vampire bats, Desmodus rotundus. Animal Behaviour, 49, 855–856.

  Dennett, D. C. (1987). The Intentional Stance. Cambridge, Mass: Bradford Books, MIT Press.

  Dennett, D. C. (1995). Darwin’s dangerous idea. New York: Simon & Schuster.

  Dennett, D. C., & Kinsbourne, M. (1992). Time and the observer: the where and when of consciousness in the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15, 183–201.

  Dere, E., et al. (2008). Animal episodic memory. In E. Dere et al. (Eds.), Handbook of episodic memory (pp. 155–184). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

  Derevianko, A. P. (2012). Recent discoveries in the Altai: Issues on the evolution of homo sapiens. Novosibirsk: RAS Press.

  de Saint-Exupéry, A. (1943). The little prince (I. Testot-Ferry, Trans.). London: Bibliophile Books.

  DeSilva, J., & Lesnik, J. (2006). Chimpanzee neonatal brain size: Implications for brain growth in Homo erectus. Journal of Human Evolution, 51, 207–212.

  Dettwyler, K. A. (1991). Can paleopathology provide evidence for compassion? American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 84, 375–384.

  de Waal, F. B. M. (1982). Chimpanzee politics. London: Jonathan Cape.

  de Waal, F. B. M. (1986). Deception in the natural communication of chimpanzees. In R. W. Mitchell & N. S. Thompson (Eds.), Deception: Perspectives on human and non-human deceit (pp. 221–244). Albany: State University of New York Press.

  de Waal, F. B. M. (1989). Food sharing and reciprocal obligations among chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 18, 433–459.

  de Waal, F. B. M. (1996). Good natured. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  de Waal, F. B. M. (2005). How animals do business. Scientific American, 292, 72–80.

  de Waal, F. B. M. (2006). Primates and philosophers: How morality evolved. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

  de Waal, F. B. M., & Aureli, F. (1996). Consolation, reconciliation, and a possible cognitive difference between macaque and chimpanzee. In A. E. Russon,

  K. A. Bard, & S. T. Parker (Eds.), Reaching into thought: The minds of the great apes (pp. 80–110). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  de Waal, F. B. M., et al. (2008). Comparing social skills of children and apes. Science, 319, 569.

  Diamond, J. (1997). Guns, germs, and steel: A short history of everybody for the last 13,000 years. New York: Simon and Schuster.

  Diamond, J. (2005). Collapse: How societies choose to fail or succeed. New York: Viking Press.

  Diamond, J. (2010). The benefits of multilingualism. Science, 330, 332–333.

  Dindo, M., et al. (2011). Observational learning in orangutan cultural transmission chains. Biology Letters, 7, 181–183.

  Dominguez-Rodrigo, M., et al. (2012). Experimental study of cut marks made with rocks unmodified by human flaking and its bearing on claims of 3.4-million-year-old butchery evidence from Dikika, Ethiopia. Journal of Archaeological Science, 39, 205–214.

  Dufour, V., & Sterck, E. H. M. (2008). Chimpanzees fail to plan in an exchange task but succeed in a tool-using procedure. Behavioural Processes, 79, 19–27.

  Dufour, V., et al. (2007). Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) anticipation of food return: Coping with waiting time in an exchange task. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 121, 145�
�155.

  Dunbar, R. I. M. (1992). Neocortex size as a constraint on group size in primates. Journal of Human Evolution, 20, 469–493.

  Dunbar, R. I. M. (1996). Grooming, gossip, and the evolution of language. London: Faber.

  Dunbar, R. I. M. (2007). Why are humans not just great apes? In C. Pasternak (Ed.), What makes us human? (pp. 37–48). Oxford: Oneworld.

  Dunbar, R. I. M. (2010). The social role of touch in humans and primates: Behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 260–268.

  Dunn, M., et al. (2011). Evolved structure of language shows lineage-specific trends in word-order universals. Nature, 473, 79–82.

  Eichenbaum, H., et al. (2005). Episodic recollection in animals: “If it walks like a duck and quacks like a duck. . . .” Learning and Motivation, 36, 190–207.

  Einstein, A. (1950). Arms can bring no security. Bulletin of the Atomic Scientist, 6, 71.

  Elston, G. N., et al. (2006). Specializations of the granular prefrontal cortex of primates: Implications for cognitive processing. Anatomical Record Part A: Discoveries in Molecular Cellular and Evolutionary Biology, 288A, 26–35.

  Emery, N. J. (2000). The eyes have it: The neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 581–604.

  Emery, N. J., & Clayton, N. S. (2004). The mentality of crows: Convergent evolution of intelligence in corvids and apes. Science, 306, 1903–1907.

  Enard, W., et al. (2002). Molecular evolution of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language. Nature, 418, 869–872.

  Epstein, R., et al. (1981). “Self-awareness” in the pigeon. Science, 212, 695–696.

  Evans, N., & Levinson, S. C. (2009). The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 429–448.

  Evans, T. A., & Beran, M. J. (2007). Chimpanzees use self-distraction to cope with impulsivity. Biology Letters, 3, 599–602.

  Everett, D. L. (2005). Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Piraha: Another look at the design features of human language. Current Anthropology, 46, 621–646.

  Fabre, J. H. (1915). The hunting wasps. New York: Dodd, Mead, and Company.

  Falk, D. (1990). Brain evolution in Homo: The radiator theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, 333–343.

  Fedor, A., et al. (2008). Object permanence tests on gibbons (Hylobatidae). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 122, 403–417.

  Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2003). The nature of human altruism. Nature, 425, 785–791.

  Fehr, E., & Gachter, S. (2002). Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature, 415, 137–140.

  Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2004). Social norms and human cooperation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 185–190.

  Fehr, E., et al. (2008). Egalitarianism in young children. Nature, 454, 1079–1083.

  Feldman, R. (2012). Oxytocin and social affiliation in humans. Hormones and Behavior, 61, 380–391.

  Feldman, R., et al. (2006). Microregulatory patterns of family interactions: Cultural pathways to toddlers’ self-regulation. Journal of Family Psychology, 20, 614–623.

  Ferrari, P. F., et al. (2006). Neonatal imitation in rhesus macaques. PLOS Biology, 4, 1501–1508.

  Fiorito, G., & Scotto, P. (1992). Observational learning in octopus vulgaris. Science, 256, 545–547.

  Fitch, W. T. (2000). The evolution of speech: A comparative review. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 258–265.

  Fitch, W. T., & Hauser, M. D. (2004). Computational constraints on syntactic processing in a nonhuman primate. Science, 303, 377–380.

  Fivush, T., et al. (2006). Elaborating on elaborations: Role of maternal reminiscing style in cognitive and socioemotional development. Child Development, 77, 1568–1588.

  Flannery, T. (1994). The future eaters: An ecological history of the Australian lands and people. New York: Grove Press.

  Flavell, J. H. (1963). The developmental psychology of Jean Piaget. New York: D. van Nostrand.

  Flavell, J. H., et al. (1983). Development of the appearance-reality distinction. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 95–120.

  Flemming, T. M., et al. (2008). What meaning means for same and different: Analogical reasoning in humans (Homo sapiens), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 122, 176–185.

  Flombaum, J. I., & Santos, L. R. (2005). Rhesus monkeys attribute perceptions to others. Current Biology, 15, 1–20.

  Flynn, J. R. (2000). IQ gains, WISC subtests and fluid g: g theory and the relevance of Spearman’s hypothesis to race. In G. R. Bock et al. (Eds.), The nature of intelligence (pp. 202–227). New York: Wiley.

  Fossey, D. (1983). Gorillas in the mist. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

  Foster, D. J., & Wilson, M. A. (2006). Reverse replay of behavioural sequences in hippocampal place cells during the awake state. Nature, 440, 680–683.

  Fouts, R. (1997). Next of kin. New York: William Morrow.

  Friedman, W. J. (2005). Developmental and cognitive perspectives on humans’ sense of the times of past and future events. Learning and Motivation, 36, 145–158.

  Frith, U., & Frith, C. (2010). The social brain: allowing humans to boldly go where no other species has been. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 365, 165–175.

  Gagnon, S., & Doré, F. Y. (1994). Cross-sectional study of object permanence in domestic puppies (Canis familiaris). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108, 220–232.

  Galdikas, B. M. F. (1980). Living with the great orange apes. National Geographic, 157, 830–853.

  Gallup, G. G. (1970). Chimpanzees: Self recognition. Science, 167, 86–87.

  Gallup, G. G. (1998). Self-awareness and the evolution of social intelligence. Behavioural Processes, 42, 239–247.

  Garcia, J., & Koelling, R. (1966). Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. Psychonomic Science, 4, 123–124.

  Garcia, J., et al. (1966). Learning with prolonged delay of reinforcement. Psychonomic Science, 5, 121–122.

  Gardner, H. (1993). Multiple intelligences: The theory in practice. New York: Basic Books.

  Gardner, R. A., & Gardner, B. T. (1969). Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee. Science, 165, 664–672.

  Garland, E. C., et al. (2011). Dynamic horizontal cultural transmission of humpback whale song at the ocean basin scale. Current Biology, 21, 687–691.

  Garrod, S., et al. (2007). Foundations of representation: Where might graphical symbol systems come from? Cognitive Science, 31, 961–987.

  Geissmann, T. (2002). Taxonomy and evolution of gibbons. Primatology and Anthropology, 11, 28–31.

  Gentner, T. Q., et al. (2006). Recursive syntactic pattern learning by songbirds. Nature, 440, 1204–1207.

  Gergely, G., et al. (2002). Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415, 755.

  Gerrans, P. (2007). Mental time travel, somatic markers and “myopia for the future.” Synthese, 159, 459–474.

  Gibbons, A. (2008). The birth of childhood. Science, 322, 1040–1043.

  Gibbons, A. (2011). African data bolster new view of modern human origins. Science, 334, 167.

  Gilbert, D. T. (2006). Stumbling on happiness. New York: A. A. Knopf.

  Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2007). Prospection: Experiencing the future. Science, 317, 1351–1354.

  Gilbert, W. S., & Sullivan, A. S. (2010). The complete plays of Gilbert and Sullivan. Rockville, MD: Wildside Press.

  Gilby, I. C. (2006). Meat sharing among the Gombe chimpanzees: Harassment and reciprocal exchange. Animal Behaviour, 71, 953–963.

  Gillan, D. J., et al. (1981). Reasoning in the chimpanzee. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Behavior Processes, 7, 1–17.

  Glickman, S., & Sroges, R. (1966). Curiosity in zoo animals. Behaviour, 26, 151–187.

  Gomes, C. M., & Boesch, C. (2009). Wild chimpanzees exchange meat for sex on a long-term basis. PLOS One, 4, e5116.

  Goodall
, J. (1964). Tool using and aimed throwing in community of free living chimpanzees. Nature, 201, 1264–1266.

  Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  Gopnik, A. (1993). How we know our minds: The illusion of first person knowledge of intentionality. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 16, 1–14.

  Gopnik, A. (2012). Scientific thinking in young children: Theoretical advances, empirical research, and policy implications. Science, 337, 1623–1627.

  Gopnik, A., & Astington, J. W. (1988). Children’s understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance-reality distinction. Child Development, 59, 26–37.

  Gordon, A. C. L., & Olson, D. R. (1998). The relation between acquisition of a theory of mind and the capacity to hold in mind. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 68, 70–83.

  Gordon, R. (1996). ‘Radical’ simulationism. In P. Carruthers & P. K. Smith (Eds.), Theories of theories of mind (pp. 11–21). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  Goren-Inbar, N., et al. (2004). Evidence of hominin control of fire at Gesher Benot Ya’aqov, Israel. Science, 304, 725–727.

  Gottfredson, L. S. (1997). Why g matters: The complexity of everyday life. Intelligence, 24, 79–132.

  Gould, S. J. (1978). Ontogeny and phylogeny. Boston, MA: Belknap.

  Gould, S. J., & Eldredge, N. (1977). Punctuated equilibria: The tempo and mode of evolution reconsidered. Paleobiology, 3, 115–151.

  Graves, R. R., et al. (2010). Just how strapping was KNM-WT 15000? Journal of Human Evolution, 59, 542–554.

  Green, R. E., et al. (2010). A draft sequence of the Neandertal genome. Science, 328, 710–722.

  Grice, H. P. (1989). Studies in the way of words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  Groves, C. P. (1989). A theory of human and primate evolution. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

  Groves, C. P. (2012a). Speciation in hominin evolution. In S. C. Reynolds & A. Gallagher (Eds.), African genesis: Perspectives on hominin evolution. (pp. 45–62). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  Groves, C. P. (2012b). Species concept in primates. American Journal of Primatology, 74, 687–691.

 

‹ Prev