Physical attractiveness can also bias the sex of your children. Now, unlike being big and tall or having a tendency toward violence, which increases the reproductive success of only men and not women, being beautiful is good for both men and women. Beautiful women have greater mating success than less attractive women, and handsome men do better than less attractive men. But beautiful men and beautiful women tend to do “better” in slightly different ways.
Physically attractive women tend to do well both in long-term and short-term mating; men prefer beautiful women for both. In contrast, handsome men tend to do well mostly in short-term mating. Women seek out handsome men for short-term mating (possibly to get good genes for their children by being impregnated by them but then passing the resulting offspring off as that of their unsuspecting husband) but not necessarily for long-term mating, for which other qualities like the man’s resources and status become more important. In fact, as we suggest in chapter 4 (see “Why Might Handsome Men Make Bad Husbands?”), physically attractive men may not make desirable long-term mates for many reasons.
So physical attractiveness, while a universally positive quality, contributes even more to women’s reproductive success than to men’s. The new hypothesis would therefore predict that physically attractive parents should have more daughters than sons. Once again, this is indeed the case. Young Americans who are rated “very attractive” have a 44 percent chance of having a son for their first child (and thus a 56 percent chance of having a daughter). In contrast, everyone else has a 52 percent chance of having a son (and thus a 48 percent chance of having a daughter) for their first child.21 Being “very attractive” increases the odds of having a daughter by 36 percent!
…and Women Are More Beautiful Than Men
If you look around and rate the men and women around you on their physical attractiveness, you should notice that, whether you are a man or a woman, gay or straight, women on average are objectively more attractive than men. Why might this be the case?
Think about it. If physical attractiveness is heritable, such that beautiful parents beget beautiful children (and less attractive parents beget similarly less attractive children), and if beautiful people are more likely to have daughters than sons, then it logically follows that over time, women will become more attractive on average than men. Once again, studies confirm this implication of the new hypothesis.22 The average level of physical attractiveness among women is significantly higher than the average level of physical attractiveness among men. Women are more beautiful than men because beautiful parents have more daughters than sons.
Far from being random chance, there are a large number of factors that appear to influence, even if only very slightly, whether a couple will have a son or a daughter. The generalized Trivers-Willard hypothesis can explain the evolutionary reasons why these factors affect the sex of the child. Evolution helps parents pass on their genes in the most efficient way possible.
Q. Why Does the Baby Have Daddy’s Eyes but Not Mommy’s?
As we discussed in chapter 2, because of the sexual asymmetries in reproductive biology, the possibility of cuckoldry exists only for men. Men can be cuckolded and unwittingly invest their limited resources in someone else’s genetic children, whereas women could never be cuckolded. In other words, paternity can never be certain, while maternity is always certain. This is well expressed in the common saying “Mommy’s baby, Daddy’s maybe.”
Men who are cuckolded do not manage to transmit their genes to the next generation and achieve no reproductive success. Men are therefore selected to be very sensitive to cues to possible cuckoldry and to attempt to guard against the possibility. A man would therefore only invest in his mate’s children if he was reasonably certain that they were genetically his. In the absence of DNA tests (which did not exist in the ancestral environment), how could men ever be certain that their children were genetically theirs?
The child’s physical resemblance would be one clue available to men in the ancestral environment. If the baby looks like the father, it is more likely that it is genetically his, whereas if the baby looks nothing like him, or, worse yet, looks a lot like his neighbor, then it is doubtful that he is its genetic father. This reasoning leads evolutionary psychologists to predict that, holding constant the probability of cuckoldry, babies who resemble their father are more likely to survive than babies who do not resemble him (or resemble the mother), because the father of babies who resemble him is more likely to be convinced of his paternity and to invest in them, thereby increasing their chances of survival, whereas the father of babies who do not resemble him (or resemble the mother) is less likely to be convinced of his paternity and to invest in them, thereby decreasing their chances of survival. Over many generations throughout evolutionary history, genes that make babies resemble the father therefore survive, whereas genes that make them resemble the mother do not, and so more and more babies come to resemble the father, until most babies are born resembling the father, not the mother.
This is precisely what two psychologists at the University of California, San Diego, Nicholas J. S. Christenfeld and Emily A. Hill, discover in their ingenious study.23 Christenfeld and Hill show the subjects in their experiment a picture of a child at ages 1, 10, and 20, and a set of three pictures of adults, one of whom is the real parent (mother or father) of the child. They then ask the subjects to match the child with the correct parent. Christenfeld and Hill’s subjects therefore have 0.33 probability of selecting the right parent by chance. If the child truly resembles the parent, then the subjects should be able to match the two pictures at a much higher probability.
A major finding in Christenfeld and Hill’s experiment is that children in general do not physically resemble their parents. The subjects are not able to match the picture of the child at any age to the picture of either the mother or the father better than expected by chance. The only exception, however, is the matching of 1-year-old babies to their father. The subjects are able to match both baby boys (0.505) and baby girls (0.480) to their father (though not to their mother) at statistically significantly greater rates than by chance. That means that one-year-old babies resemble their fathers, as might be expected from the evolutionary psychological logic presented above.
Christenfeld and Hill’s finding was widely reported in the media, but it has also become one of the most controversial contentions in evolutionary psychology, not least because, although their explanation had impeccable logic, their finding could not be replicated. To date, attempts at replication have shown that newborn babies objectively resemble mothers more than fathers,24 and infants and children resemble both parents equally.25 Thus, the question of whether newborn babies objectively resemble the father more than the mother must be treated as an open one until more experiments are conducted.
But Who Are Newborn Babies Said to Resemble?
There is a related finding that is much less controversial and well replicated, however. Nature may or may not help assure fathers of their paternity by making babies resemble them; however, friends and family—in particular, mothers and their kin—certainly do. In three separate studies conducted in three different North American countries (Canada, Mexico, and the United States) in three different decades, mothers and maternal relatives are far more likely to allege the baby’s paternal resemblance than its maternal resemblance.26 This happens even when the newborn babies in fact do not resemble their fathers.27 Such allegations of paternal resemblance assure the fathers of their paternity, whether the babies actually resemble them or not.
Further, in most societies, babies get their last name from the father, not the mother, thereby once again suggesting to the father that he is the father of the baby. (Russians go one step further and give their babies their middle and last names after the father.) This is true even in societies where women routinely keep their last name when they get married and do not adopt their husband’s name. The children of such parents nonetheless usually get their last name from th
e father, not the mother. Many Western professional women these days often keep their last name when they get married. Most of their children still get their last names from the father, not the mother. By giving their children the father’s last name, these women are essentially (albeit unconsciously) saying, “Honey, it’s yours” (even, or especially, when it is not). They need to reassure their husbands of their paternity, but do not themselves need to be reassured of their maternity; they know for sure. Mommy’s baby, Daddy’s maybe. And for 10–30 percent of daddies, it is not.
Q. Why Are There So Many Deadbeat Dads but So Few Deadbeat Moms?
When married couples with children get divorced, chances are that the children stay with the mother, not the father, especially when they are young. According to the 1992 March/April Current Population Survey in the United States, conducted by the US Census Bureau on a nationally representative sample, 86 percent of custodial parents are mothers.28 Further, many of the noncustodial fathers who have agreed, either voluntarily or via court order, to pay child support default on their commitment and often become “deadbeat dads.” The first national survey of the receipt of child support, conducted in 1978, reveals that less than half (49 percent) of women awarded child support actually receive the full amount due to them, and more than a quarter (28 percent) of them receive nothing.29 The percentages have remained more or less constant since. In 1991, 52 percent of custodial parents awarded child support received the full amount; 25 percent of them received nothing.30 Why are women so much more dedicated parents than men? Why are there so many deadbeat dads but so few deadbeat moms?
On the surface, this massive sex difference in the dedication to parenthood might appear puzzling, since both the mother and the father are equally related to their children genetically; each transmits half of their genes to their child. However, there are two biological factors that combine to make fathers far less committed as parents than mothers.
“Mommy’s Baby, Daddy’s Maybe”
The first is paternity uncertainty. As we discussed in chapter 2 and above in this chapter (“Why Does the Baby Have Daddy’s Eyes but Not Mommy’s?”), because gestation for all mammals (including humans) takes place internally within the female’s body, the male can never be certain of his paternity, whereas maternity is always certain. And paternity uncertainty is not a remote theoretical possibility. As we mention elsewhere (see “The Evolutionary Psychological Perspective” in chapter 1 and “Why Are There Virtually No Polyandrous Societies?” in chapter 4), the estimated incidences of cuckoldry (men unwittingly investing in another man’s genetic offspring) in contemporary industrialized societies is substantial (between 10 and 30 percent), although a comprehensive recent review suggests that the actual incidence among Western populations may be much lower, at around 4 percent.31 Thus, this is a very realistic possibility for any father in contemporary Western society (and probably elsewhere throughout history as well). Naturally, men are not motivated to invest in children who have a distinct possibility of not being genetically theirs.
The Best They Can Do Is Better for Men
The second biological factor that makes fathers less committed parents is their higher fitness ceiling (the best they can do reproductively). Fetuses gestate for nine months within the female body, and infants are, at least in the past, nursed by the mother for several years after birth, during which the mother is usually infertile. Women also have a much shorter reproductive life than men do. These two factors combine to create a much higher fitness ceiling for men than for women. Men can potentially have many more children than women can. (Remember Moulay Ismail the Bloodthirsty?) The sex difference in the largest possible number of children means that, while reproductive success is equally important to men and women (in fact, to all biological organisms), each child is far more important to the mother than it is to the father. Each child represents a far greater portion of a woman’s lifetime reproductive potential than it does a man’s. If a 40-year-old mother of five deserts her children and they die as a result, she will likely end her life as a total reproductive loser, having failed to leave any copy of her genes in the next generation. If a 40-year-old father of five does the same, he can go on to produce five (or ten or twenty) more children.
Both paternity uncertainty and the higher fitness ceiling make fathers less committed parents than mothers, and this is why there are so many more deadbeat dads than deadbeat moms; very few women abandon or neglect their children. Ironically, it is the mother’s greater commitment to her children that allows the father to neglect them even more.32 Knowing the mother’s greater commitment to her children, the father can abandon them, secure in the knowledge that the mother would never do likewise, because if she did, the children would be virtually certain to die. In other words, divorced parents with children are playing a game of chicken, and it is usually the mother who swerves. Most fathers would probably prefer to invest in their children and raise them by themselves rather than see them die, but they normally do not have to make this difficult decision, because they know that the mother would never abandon them. The mother’s greater commitment to her children ironically allows the father to have his cake and eat it too, by moving on to the next marriage and family in which to invest.
Are Mothers Always Good Parents?
None of this means that all mothers are always good parents or better parents than fathers. Sometimes mothers even kill their babies. However, evolutionary psychological logic can even explain who is more likely to kill their babies, and why.
Statistics show that very young mothers, by far, are the most likely to kill their babies, and older mothers are the second most likely to do so, but for different reasons.33 Very young, teenage mothers kill their babies because they still have most of their reproductive lives ahead of them, and they can make more babies in the future even if they kill the one they just had. Having a baby under unfortunate circumstances (such as without the father willing to invest in it) not only threatens the well-being of the baby but also jeopardizes the mother’s chance of finding a mate in the future. And teenage mothers are more likely to have their baby under unfortunate circumstances than others.
Older mothers (above the age of 35) kill their babies for a different reason. They are more likely to have defective babies because of their age. Every child (defective or otherwise) consumes parents’ resources. Since defective children are much less likely to attain reproductive success, from the purely genetic point of view, any resources invested in children who will not have children themselves are wasted. Such children are taking away valuable resources from other children who have better reproductive prospects. Older mothers are more likely than younger mothers to have other children they must also raise. So parents are designed not to invest in defective children. (By the same token, parents invest more in better-looking children than in less good-looking children, and in more intelligent children than in less intelligent children.)
Yes, the evolutionary logic is very brutal, cold, and heartless. It only cares about the survival of the genes.
Q. Why Is Family More Important to Women Than to Men?
Ask a group of friends, colleagues, and acquaintances (both men and women) to name five of their closest associates. Who are the people they talk to when they have something important to discuss? Chances are that women in your circles mention more family members among their closest associates, whereas men mention more coworkers and business associates in their personal networks. Studies in social networks repeatedly find that while otherwise comparable men and women have similar personal networks, women typically have more kin and fewer coworkers than men do.34 Why is this? Why are women closer to their family members than men are?
Two sociologists, Lynn Smith-Lovin and J. Miller McPherson, propose an explanation for this universal phenomenon from the Standard Social Science Model perspective.35 Using fictitious characters named Jim and Jane, they explain how the compositions of their personal networks remain more or less the same through
adult years because “Jim is serious about his career as an engineer [and] Jane is equally serious about her nursing.” However, the change begins when they become parents. “When their first child is born, however, Jane’s mother comes to visit for two weeks; Jane begins to use her sister as a babysitter for daytime care while she is working…. Because more of her time is taken up with the baby, Jane’s networks become more centered on neighborhood and kin, to some extent at the expense of her work and voluntary association friends. Jim’s work and group ties are less altered.”36
Jane and Jim in Smith-Lovin and McPherson’s description accurately mirror what happens to many young couples when they have children. So, in that sense, their explanation is correct. However, it simply begs the question: Why is it Jane’s mother, not Jim’s, who comes to visit after the baby is born, when Jim’s mother is presumably as equally related to the baby as Jane’s mother? Or is she? Why is it Jane’s sister, not Jim’s, who becomes their babysitter, when both sisters are equally related to the baby? Or are they? Smith-Lovin and McPherson assume that it is Jane, not Jim, who is the primary caretaker of the baby. Why is this so? Their Standard Social Science Model explanation cannot answer these fundamental questions. (We will return to another fundamental question of why Jim is an engineer and Jane is a nurse in chapter 7, “Why Are Most Neurosurgeons Male and Most Kindergarten Teachers Female?”)
Evolutionary psychology can answer all of these questions. We have already addressed why mothers (like Jane) are more committed to their children than fathers (like Jim) are. (See “Why Are There So Many Deadbeat Dads but So Few Deadbeat Moms?” above.) So we know why Jane becomes the primary caretaker of the baby, not Jim (who may or may not be the baby’s genetic father and who, if he becomes a successful engineer, might leave Jane and their child when Jane is 40 and marry the 20-year-old receptionist he hired for his engineering firm and start a new family anyway). Even though women are more motivated to make parental investment than men are, they cannot always do it alone; sometimes they need help from others. This was especially true back in the ancestral environment, where resources were scarce and life was precarious.
Why Beautiful People Have More Daughters: From Dating, Shopping, and Praying to Going to War and Becoming a Billionaire–Two Evolutionary Psychologists Explain Why We Do What We Do Page 11