Asian Traditions of Meditation

Home > Other > Asian Traditions of Meditation > Page 6
Asian Traditions of Meditation Page 6

by Halvor Eifring


  Davidson, Richard J. “Empirical Explorations of Mindfulness: Conceptual and Methodological Conundrums.” Emotion 10, no. 1 (2010): 8–11.

  Du Bois, John W. “Co-Opting Intersubjectivity: Dialogic Rhetoric of the Self.” In The Rhetorical Emergence of Culture, edited by Christian Meyer and Felix Girke, 52–83. New York: Berghahn Books, 2011.

  Eifring, Halvor, ed. Hindu, Buddhist and Daoist Meditation: Cultural Histories. Oslo: Hermes, 2014.

  ———, ed. Meditation and Culture: The Interplay of Practice and Context. London: Bloomsbury Academic, 2015.

  ———, ed. Meditation in Judaism, Christianity and Islam: Cultural Histories. London: Bloomsbury Academic, 2013.

  ———. “Meditation in Judaism, Christianity and Islam: Technical Aspects of Devotional Practices.” In Eifring, Meditation in Judaism, Christianity and Islam, 3–13.

  ———. “Meditative Pluralism in Hānshān Déqīng.” In Eifring, Meditation and Culture, 102–127.

  ———. “Spontaneous Thoughts in Meditative Traditions.” In Eifring, Meditation and Culture, 200–215.

  Eifring, Halvor, and Are Holen. “The Uses of Attention: Elements of Meditative Practice.” In Eifring, Hindu, Buddhist and Daoist Meditation, 1–26.

  Eliade, Mircea. Yoga: Immortality and Freedom. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press, 1969.

  Feuerstein, Georg. Holy Madness: Spirituality, Crazy-Wise Teachers, and Enlightenment. Revised and expanded edition. Prescott, AZ: Hohm Press, 2006.

  ———. Tantra: The Path of Ecstasy. Boston: Shambhala Publications, 1998.

  ———. Yoga: The Technology of Ecstasy. Los Angeles: J. P. Tarcher, 1989.

  Fischer, Roland. “A Cartography of the Ecstatic and Meditative States.” Science 26 (1971): 897–904.

  Forman, Robert K. C., ed. The Problem of Pure Consciousness. New York: Oxford University Press, 1990.

  Geers, A. L., P. E. Weiland, K. Kosbab, S. J. Landry, and S. G. Helfer. “Goal Activation, Expectations, and the Placebo Effect.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 89, no. 2 (2005): 143–159.

  Geers, A. L., S. G. Helfer, P. E. Weiland, and K. Kosbab. “Expectations and Placebo Response: A Laboratory Investigation into the Role of Somatic Focus.” Journal of Behavioral Medicine 29, no. 2 (2006): 171–178.

  Goleman, Daniel J. “Meditation and Consciousness: An Asian Approach to Mental Health.” American Journal of Psychiatry 30, no. 1 (1976): 41–54.

  ———. The Meditative Mind: The Varieties of Meditative Experience. Los Angeles: Jeremy P. Tarcher / Perigree Books, 1988.

  Halbfass, Wilhelm. “The Concept of Experience in the Encounter between India and the West.” In India and Europe: An Essay in Understanding, edited by Wilhelm Halbfass, 378–402. Albany, NY: SUNY Press, 1988.

  Handelman, Don. “Postlude: The Interior Sociality of Self-Transformation.” In Shulman and Stroumsa, Self and Self-Transformation, 236–253.

  Hasenkamp, W., C. D. Wilson-Mendenhall, E. Duncan, and L. W. Barsalou. “Mind Wandering and Attention during Focused Meditation: A Fine-Grained Temporal Analysis of Fluctuating Cognitive States.” Neuroimage 59 (2012): 750–760.

  Hodgson, Phyllis, ed. The Cloud of Unknowing and the Book of Privy Counselling. London: Oxford University Press, 1973.

  ———, ed. Deonise Hid Diuinite, and Other Treatises on Contemplative Prayer Related to “The Cloud of Unknowing.” London: Oxford University Press, 1958.

  Hofmann, S. G., P. Grossman, and D. E. Hinton. “Loving-Kindness and Compassion Meditation: Potential for Psychological Interventions.” Clinical Psychology Review 31 (2011): 1126–1132.

  Holen, Are. Inner Strength: The Free Mental Attitude in Acem Meditation. 2nd ed. Oslo: Acem Publishing, 2006.

  Holzel, Britta K., Ulrich Ott, Hannes Hempel, Andrea Hackl, Katharina Wolf, Rudolf Stark, and Dieter Vaitl. “Differential Engagement of Anterior Cingulate and Adjacent Medial Frontal Cortex in Adept Meditators and Non-Meditators.” Neuroscience Letters 421 (2007): 16–21.

  Ivanov, Sergey A. Holy Fools in Byzantium and Beyond. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2006.

  Kabat-Zinn, Jon. Coming to Our Senses: Healing Ourselves and the World through Mindfulness. New York: Hyperion, 2005.

  Kapstein, Matthew T. “Rethinking Religious Experience: Seeing the Light in the History of Religions.” In The Presence of Light: Divine Radiance and Religious Experience, edited by Matthew T. Kapstein, 265–299. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press, 2004.

  Katz, Steven T., ed. Mysticism and Philosophical Analysis. New York: Oxford University Press, 1978.

  Kennedy, Brian, and Elizabeth Guo. Chinese Martial Arts Training Manuals: A Historical Survey. Berkeley, CA: Blue Snake Books, 2005.

  Kirsch, I. “Response Expectancy as a Determinant of Experience and Behavior.” American Psychologist 40, no. 11 (1985): 1189–1202.

  Kohn, Livia. Meditation Works: In the Hindu, Buddhist and Daoist Traditions. Magdalena, NM: Three Pines Press, 2008.

  Kokoszka, A. “Axiological Aspects of Comparing Psychotherapy and Meditation.” International Journal of Psychosomatics 37 (1990): 78–81.

  Komjathy, Louis. Cultivating Perfection: Mysticism and Self-Transformation in Early Quanzhen Daoism. Leiden: Brill, 2007.

  Larchet, Jean-Claude. Mental Disorders and Spiritual Healing: Teachings from the Early Christian East. Translated by G. John Champoux and Rama P. Coomaraswamy. San Rafael, CA: Angelico Press, 2011.

  Lazar, Sara W., George Bush, Randy L. Gollub, Gregory L. Fricchione, Gurucharan Khalsa, and Herbert Benson. “Functional Brain Mapping of the Relaxation Response and Meditation.” NeuroReport 11, no. 7 (2000): 1581–1585.

  Linde, K., C. M. Witt, A. Streng, W. Weidenhammer, S. Wagenpfeil, B. Brinkhaus, S. N. Willich, and D. Melchart. “The Effect of Patient Expectations on Outcomes in Four Randomized Controlled Trials of Acupuncture in Patients with Chronic Pain.” Pain 128, no. 3 (2007): 264–271.

  Linrothe, Rob, ed. Holy Madness: Portraits of Tantric Siddhas. New York: Rubin Museum of Art.

  Loori, John Daido, ed. The Art of Just Sitting: Essential Writings on the Zen Practice of Shikantaza. Boston: Wisdom Publications, 2002.

  Manna, A., A. Raffone, M. G. Perrucci, D. Nardo, A. Ferretti, A. Tartaro, A. Londei, C. Del Gratta, M. O. Belardinelli, and G. L. Romani. “Neural Correlates of Focused Attention and Cognitive Monitoring in Meditation.” Brain Research Bulletin 82, nos. 1–2 (March 2010): 46–56.

  McDaniel, June. The Madness of the Saints: Ecstatic Religion in Bengal. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 1989.

  Nanamoli, Bhikkhu, trans. The Middle Length Discourses of the Buddha: A New Translation of the Majjhima Nikaya. Edited by Bhikkhu Bodhi. Boston: Wisdom Publications, 1995.

  Needham, Rodney. “Polythetic Classification: Convergence and Consequences.” Man: New Series 10, no. 3 (1975): 349–369.

  Osho. Meditation: The Art of Ecstasy. Pune, India: Tao Publishing, 2006.

  Ospina, Maria B., Kenneth Bond, Mohammad Karkhaneh, Lisa Tjosvold, Ben Vandermeer, Yuanyuan Liang, Liza Bialy, Nicola Hooton, Nina Buscemi, Donna M. Dryden, and Terry P. Klassen. “Meditation Practices for Health: State of the Research.” Evidence Report/Technology Assessment, no. 155. Rockville, MD: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality.

  Perez de Albeniz, A., and J. Holmes. “Meditation: Concepts, Effects and Uses in Therapy.” International Journal of Psychotherapy 5, no. 1 (2000): 49–58.

  Philippe, Jacques. Time for God. New York: Scepter Publishers, 2008.

  Raposa, Michael J. Meditation and the Martial Arts. Charlottesville: University of Virginia Press, 2003.

  Robinet, Isabelle. Taoist Meditation: The Mao-Shan Tradition of Great Purity. Translated by Julian F. Pas and Norman J. Girardot. New York: SUNY Press, 1993.

  Rydell-Johnsén, Henrik. “The Early Jesus Prayer and Meditation in Greco-Roman Philosophy.” In Eifring, Meditation in Judaism, Christianity and Islam, 93–106.

  Satlow, Michael L. “Defining Judaism: Accounting for ‘Religions�
� in the Study of Religion.” Journal of the American Academy of Religion 74, no. 4 (2006): 837–860.

  Seppälä, Serafim. “Meditation in the East Syrian Tradition.” In Eifring, Meditation in Judaism, Christianity and Islam, 107–121.

  Shahar, Meir. The Shaolin Monastery: History, Religion, and the Martial Arts. Honolulu: University of Hawai‘i Press, 2008.

  Sharf, Robert. “Buddhist Modernism and the Rhetoric of Meditative Experience.” Numen 42, no. 3 (1995): 228–283.

  Shaw, Sarah. Buddhist Meditation: An Anthology of Texts form the Pāli Canon. London: Routledge, 2006.

  Sheng Yen. The Method of No Method: The Chan Practice of Silent Illumination. Boston: Shambhala, 2008.

  Shulman, David, and Guy G. Stroumsa. “Introduction: Persons, Passages, and Shifting Cultural Space.” In Shulman and Stroumsa, Self and Self-Transformation, 1–16.

  ———, eds. Self and Self-Transformation in the History of Religions. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2002.

  Smith, Jonathan Z. Imagining Religion: From Babylon to Jonestown. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1982.

  ———. Relating Religion: Essays in the Study of Religion. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 2004.

  Strickman, Michel. Chinese Magical Medicine. Edited by Bernard Faure. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press, 2002.

  Trenckner, V., ed. The Majjhima-nikaya. Vol. 1. London: Pali Text Society / Henry Frowde, 1888.

  van der Toorn, Karel. Review of Relating Religion, by Jonathan Z. Smith. Journal of Biblical Literature 124, no. 3 (2005): 584–587.

  Wallace, R. K., H. Benson, and A. F. Wilson. “A Wakeful Hypometabolic Physiologic State.” American Journal of Physiology 221, no. 3 (1971): 795–799.

  Walsh, Roger, and Shauna L. Shapiro. “The Meeting of Meditative Disciplines and Western Psychology: A Mutually Enriching Dialogue.” American Psychologist 61, no. 3 (2006): 227–239.

  West, M. “Meditation.” British Journal of Psychiatry 135 (1979): 457–467.

  Wolters, Clifton, trans. The Cloud of Unknowing and Other Works. London: Penguin Books, 1978.

  Xu, Jian, Alexandra Vik, Inge R. Groote, Jim Lagopoulos, Are Holen, Øyvind Ellingsen, Asta K. Håberg, and Svend Davanger. “Nondirective Meditation Activates Default Mode Network and Areas Associated with Memory Retrieval and Emotional Processing.” Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8 (2014): article 86, 1–10.

  Xūyún lǎo héshàng niánpǔ fǎhuì zēngdìngběn. Taipei: Shífāng dàjuésì, 1986.

  2 HALVOR EIFRING

  Types of Meditation

  Since the 1970s, both scientific and popular literature in the West have tended to classify meditation techniques on the basis of the Buddhist dichotomy between samatha and vipassanā, often rendered in English as “concentration” and “insight” (or “mindfulness”),1 and more recently termed “focused attention” and “open monitoring.”2 The interpretation of this distinction, however, varies from one writer to the next and has proven to be quite problematic. Before the twentieth century, the distinction had little impact outside Buddhism, apart from its early borrowing and transformation within Chinese Daoism.3 Even within Buddhism, the distinction was always ambiguous and controversial. More importantly, quite a few scholars have pointed to modern forms of meditation that do not fall neatly within either category, and this problem is exacerbated when, as in this volume, we include a broader spectrum of traditional meditation practices.

  This chapter will explore an alternative classification into “directive” and “nondirective” practices, which in recent years has been suggested by a number of scientists studying the effects of meditation. In our context, the distinction may be defined as follows:4

  Directive techniques seek to lead the mind and body toward preset goals embedded in culturally determined webs of meaning, while nondirective techniques are based on universal psychobiological working mechanisms bringing about reflexive effects on mind and body.

  Unlike the Buddhist dichotomy, this classification does not purport to reflect traditional concepts. It is etic not emic, and thus transcends the division lines between various traditions. This chapter will attempt to explore the relevance of the distinction for both modern and traditional meditation practices across the Eurasian continent.

  The chapter builds on three types of source material. First, it refers to other contributions in this volume and other volumes in the Cultural Histories of Meditation project. Second, it refers to modern discussions of the classification of meditative practices, including various scientific attempts. And third, it refers to a number of texts on meditation from various contemplative traditions, in particular the dharma texts (fǎ-yǔ) of the Chinese Buddhist meditation master Hānshān Déqīng (1546–1623) and “The Cloud of Unknowing” by an anonymous fourteenth-century English country parson.

  Directive and Nondirective Elements

  The extent to which meditation seeks to direct the person toward preset goals largely depends on the degree of active interference with the cognitive and emotional content of the mind. Such interference may lie in the context in which meditation is learned or practiced, with suggestive elements bringing about expectations of particular states of mind, visions of deities, healing effects, and so on. It may also lie in the mental attitude with which meditation is practiced, in the form of attempts at excluding peripheral or digressive thoughts, in order to concentrate fully on the meditation object. Or it may lie in the meditation object itself, as when the thematic content of the meditation object leads the cognitive or emotional content of the mind in predetermined directions. Conversely, nondirective meditation seeks to reduce suggestive elements in the context, to accept the presence of peripheral and digressive thoughts, and to use meditation objects without thematic content. See table 2.1.

  The different elements may be freely combined, so that, for instance, a concentrative mental attitude may be combined with a nonthematic meditation object, or vice versa. The setting usually contains both suggestive and nonsuggestive elements. The contrast between directive and nondirective meditation, therefore, is a question of degrees, not of absolute opposites.

  While the context is obviously important and needs to be studied, this chapter will focus on the role of the elements that are part of the meditation technique, that is, the mental attitude and the meditation object. The object of meditation is the focus of attention during meditation, the element toward which attention is intentionally directed, while the mental attitude is the mode of attention during meditation, characterized by the relation between the focal element (meditation object) and elements that are either peripheral (background thoughts or impressions) or unintentionally focal (digressions, as when the object of meditation is temporarily forgotten and random thoughts take over).

  Table 2.1. Directive and Nondirective Forms of Meditation

  Directive meditation Nondirective meditation

  Context suggestive nonsuggestive

  Mental attitude concentrative nonconcentrative

  Meditation object thematic nonthematic

  The distinction between directive and nondirective forms of meditation reflects two ways in which meditative practice may bring about inner transformation. On the one hand, such change may be an outside-in process in which the context plays a dominant role, as when the meditative effect of mantras comes from their place within the cosmologies surrounding them that endow them with symbolic if not literal meaning and thus help meditators to “‘discover’ the knowledge already cultivated by their traditions.”5 On the other hand, meditative change may be an inside-out process that starts in the mind and body of the meditator, as when the effects of mantra-based meditation come from a psychobiological “relaxation response” triggered by the “repetition of a … sound … or muscular activity” combined with a mental attitude of “passively disregarding everyday thoughts that inevitably come to mind and returning to your repetition,”6 without any conceptual or symbolic meaning being involved. Directive meditation fosters outsid
e-in processes, while non-directive meditation fosters inside-out processes. Most often, the two are combined in various proportions.7 Toward the end of this chapter, we shall return to the question of whether directive and nondirective forms of meditation constitute different paths toward the same goal or actually produce different effects.

  The distinction between directive and nondirective forms of meditation is also reflected in the contrast between content and process orientation. As mentioned in the previous chapter in this volume, some scholars argue that all meditation emphasizes “process rather than content,”8 in contrast to nonmeditative practices such as self-hypnosis and psychotherapy, which “aim primarily at changing mental contents … such as thoughts, images, and emotions.”9 While this may be true of nondirective meditation, and therefore to a large extent of the modern practices studied by scientists, many traditional forms of meditation are directive and aim at “changing mental contents.”10

  The Problems with Samatha and Vipassanā

  At the outset, samatha and vipassanā (or “concentration” and “insight,” etc.) are Buddhist terms. The modern discourse on the distinction between the two is largely a product of Western scientific attempts at interpreting these traditional terms, and their English near-equivalents, and applying them to the various meditation practices that figure prominently in scientific studies, or that have been popular in Europe and America in the last fifty years. Both the terms and the practices to which they have been applied have their origins in Asia, but have been largely dislodged from their original contexts. Furthermore, while the terms are rooted in Buddhism, many of the meditation methods to which they are applied hail from other Asian traditions.

  The modern discourse on this distinction differs considerably from traditional Buddhist notions, according to which samatha seeks stillness and absorption, while vipassanā seeks insight and wisdom. In the modern context, the distinction is typically no longer based on the purpose of meditation but on its technical features. In brief, the mental attitude corresponding to samatha seeks an exclusive focus on the meditation object, while the attitude corresponding to vipassanā cultivates an open awareness toward all impressions, without any specific meditation object.11 See table 2.2.

 

‹ Prev