Elephant Sense and Sensibility

Home > Other > Elephant Sense and Sensibility > Page 18
Elephant Sense and Sensibility Page 18

by Michael Garstang


  itors, the Maasai, conflict between the two is rare and triggered most often when

  events like extreme drought stress both populations. In the Amboseli-Tsavo

  Group Ranch Conservation area between 1993 and 2005, 18 people were killed

  and the same number injured by elephants (Moss and Lee, 2011, p. 331). Over

  nearly the same period of time (1997–2007), 91 elephants were speared, 57 of

  which died.

  Them and Us Chapter | 14 115

  FIGURE 14.4 Expanded Addo Elephant National Park. (1) Original Addo elephant enclosure (approximately 220 km2 or 80 mile2); (2) inland expansion (approximately 1200 km2 or 430 mile2); and (3) coastal parkland and ocean including Stag and Bird Island (approximately 1400 km2 or 500 mile2). (See also Figure 1.2 in Chapter 1.)

  TABLE 14.1 Annual numbers of human deaths caused by elephants in protected

  areas, communal lands, and enterprises using tame elephants (e.g., elephant

  theme parks and elephant-back safari operations) in South Africa

  Protected

  Communal

  Enterprise Using

  Year

  Areas

  Land

  Tame Elephants

  Total

  2002

  2

  1

  0

  3

  2003

  3

  2

  0

  5

  2004

  2

  0

  0

  2

  2005

  4

  0

  1

  5

  2006

  2

  0

  1

  3

  2007

  0

  1

  0

  1

  Total number

  13

  4

  2

  19

  Percent

  60%

  21%

  11%

  After Scholes and Mennell (2008, p. 222). Permission courtesy of the University of the Witwatersrand Press and the authors Scholes and Mennell.

  116 Elephant Sense and Sensibility

  Despite the close contact and resulting conflict, when resources are shared

  elephants have not aggressively attacked humans. In fact, as shown earlier, they

  recognize and can discriminate between humans who pose a threat and those

  who do not. Their response is to avoid rather than seek conflict. In the Amboseli

  study, which through force of circumstances has had to deal with elephant–

  people conflict, solutions have been found and the prospects for mutual survival

  exist, albeit resting in the hands of humans.

  The second possible hope that humans and elephants can coexist lies in the

  evolutionary history of humans and elephants. For a far greater part of this his-

  tory or about 99.99% of the time, humans and elephants have coexisted with

  little or no conflict. In fact, humans probably feared elephants much more than

  the other way around. Only for a fraction of their mutual history, no more than

  one-hundredth of one percent of the time, were humans and elephants in mortal

  combat. It may be just possible that with the smallest sign of tolerance exhibited

  by humans for the well-being of elephants, it will be the elephants that meet us

  more than halfway.

  Both solutions, however, may fail in the face of growing inhumanity and

  human greed. Unless a concerted international response is mounted within the

  immediate future, there is a real danger that the poaching of elephants and rhi-

  noceros, for their tusks and horns, will eradicate both species from the wild.

  Poaching must be viewed by the world’s community in the identical light

  as we would a crime against humanity. We must see the slaughter of elephants,

  and other wildlife, with the same horror that we view ethnic cleansing. This

  slaughter of animals when seen in proportion to the global numbers involved far

  exceeds any assault humans have ever perpetrated upon their own kind.

  To halt this Faustian tragedy, the international community, including those

  nations implicated in the crime, needs to take action simultaneously on two

  fronts: ban the sale of ivory and horn and vigorously and effectively suppress

  poaching.

  Both actions have challenged global conservation forces in the past.

  Banning the sale of ivory has been effective; suppressing poaching much less

  so. International action can severely curtail the sale of ivory. Stopping poaching

  will require an advanced, well-equipped task force. Such a special force cannot

  be deployed simultaneously in all regions where poaching is rife. Instead, it

  should be deployed in a limited area where the chances of success are high, and,

  more importantly, the message to all poachers and those who support them will

  be the strongest. Once the suppression of poaching in one region has succeeded,

  methodology, personnel, and equipment can be transferred to cover a second

  selected region. Determined action of this kind would, in conjunction with the

  enforcement of trade agreements, rapidly reduce the slaughter to sustainable

  levels. If the account of elephants in this book is to have any meaning, then it

  calls upon our species to act with determination on behalf of our sorely tried

  brethren.

  Them and Us Chapter | 14 117

  Having done so, however, the battle will not have been won. Rather, the real

  battle for mutual survival on this planet can then begin in earnest. This battle

  will be to confront the combined demands of a growing global human popula-

  tion and its inevitable drain upon the finite resources of the planet. Our inability

  to limit contamination and pollution of the fluid systems of the earth, air, and

  water is evident in almost every corner of the world. Careful studies in regions

  that, until very recently, were thought to be immune or too remote to suffer any

  serious degradation are now showing disturbing responses in vegetation and

  species degeneration. Hutto (2014) has shown that mule deer, mountain goats,

  and elk in remote regions of the high Rockies may all be in an irreversible

  decline.

  Africa, despite environmental limitations discussed earlier, is seen by global

  and indigenous economists as the continent of the future. To hope that humans,

  in this diverse ecological continent, will be persuaded by the costly lessons we

  have learned to seek some equilibrium in which species other than ourselves

  might survive or even prosper is perhaps to hope in vain. All that has been re-

  counted in this book, however, denies capitulation. At some point with this and

  many, many more efforts we, and elephants, as sentient beings, must prevail.

  References

  Anthony, L., Spence, G., 2009. The Elephant Whisperer: My Life with the Herd in the African Wild.

  Thomas Dunne Books, St. Martin’s Press, New York, 368 p.

  Bates, L.A., et al., 2007. Elephants classify human ethnic groups by odor and garment color. Curr.

  Biol. 17, 1938–1942.

  Bates, L.A., et al., 2008a. Do elephants show empathy? J. Concious. Stud. 15, 204–225.

  Bates, L.A., et al., 2008b. African elephants have expectations of out-of-sight family members.

  Biol. Lett. 4, 34–36.

  Beaune, D., et al., 2013. Seed dispersal strategies and the threat of defaunation in a Congo forest.

  Biodivers. Conserv. 22, 225–238.

/>   Bekoff, M., 2002. Minding Animals: Awareness, Emotions and Heart. Oxford University Press,

  Oxford.

  Bekoff, M., Pierce, J., 2009. Wild Justice: The Moral Lives of Animals. University of Chicago

  Press, Chicago, 204 p.

  Bell, W.D.M., 1960. Bell of Africa. Neville Spearman and The Holland Press, London, 228 p.

  Bradshaw, G.A., 2009. Elephants on the Edge: What Animals Teach Us About Humanity. Sheridan

  Books, Ann Arbor, MI, 310 p.

  Brenner, E., et al., 2006. Plant neurobiology: an integrated view of plant signaling. Trends Plant

  Sci. 11, 413–419.

  Brown, C.H., 1994. Sound localization. In: Fay, R.R., Popper, A.N. (Eds.), Comparative Hearing:

  Mammals. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, New York, pp. 57–96.

  Buckley, C., 2009. Tarra & Bella. G.P. Putnam’s Sons, Penguin Group, Inc., New York.

  Byrne, R., 1997. What’s the use of anecdotes? Distinguishing psychological mechanisms in pri-

  mate tactical deception. In: Mitchell, R.W., Thompson, N.S., Miles, H.L. (Eds.), Anthropomor-

  phism, Anecdotes, and Animals. State University of New York Press, New York, pp. 134–150

  (chapter 12).

  Byrne, R., 2006. Animal cognition: know your enemy. Curr. Biol. 16, R686–R688.

  Byrne, R., Bates, L., 2006. Why are animals cognitive? Curr. Biol. 16, R445–R448.

  Byrne, R., Bates, L., 2009. Elephant cognition in primate perspective. Comp. Cogn. Behav. Rev.

  4, 65–79.

  Byrne, R., Bates, L., 2010. Primate social cognition: uniquely primate, uniquely social or just

  unique. Neuron 65, 815–830.

  Byrne, R., Bates, L., 2011a. Elephant cognition: what we know about what elephants know. In:

  Moss, C., Croze, H., Lee, P. (Eds.), The Amboseli Elephants: A Long-Term Perspective of a

  Long-Lived Animal. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 174–181 (chapter 11).

  Byrne, R., Bates, L., 2011b. Cognition in the wild: exploring animal minds with observational

  evidence. Biol. Lett. 7, 619–622.

  Byrne, R.W., et al., 2009. How did they get here from there? Detecting changes in direction in ter-

  restrial ranging. Anim. Behav. 77, 619–631.

  119

  120 References

  Campos-Arceiz, A., Blake, S., 2011. Megagardeners of the forest—the role of elephants in seed

  dispersal. Acta Oecol. 37, 542–553.

  Caro, T.M., Hauser, M.D., 1992. Is there teaching in non-human animals? Q. Rev. Biol. 67,

  151–174.

  Carter, P., 2008. The elephants of Mfuwe Lodge. http://www.africatravelguide.com/articles/the-

  elephants-of-mfuwe-lodge.html.

  Chase, M., 2007. Elephants learn to avoid land mines in war torn Angola. Reported by L. Marshall, National Geographic News. NationalGeographic.comwww.elephantswithoutborders.org.

  Chase, M., Griffin, C., 2011. Elephants of southeast Angola in war and peace: their decline, re-

  colonization and recent status. Afr. J. Ecol. 49, 353–361.

  Chiyo, P.I., et al., 2011. Using molecular and observational techniques to estimate the number and

  raiding patterns of crop-raiding elephants. J. Appl. Ecol. 48, 788–796.

  Christo, C., Wilkinson, W., 2009. Walking Thunder: In the Footsteps of the African Elephant. Mer-

  rill Publishers Ltd, London, New York, 158 p.

  Clemins, P.J., et al., 2005. Automatic classification and speaker identification of African elephant

  ( Loxodonta africana) vocalizations. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 117, 1–8.

  Cochrane, E.P., 2003. The need to be eaten: Balanites wilsoniana with and without elephant seed-

  dispersal. J. Trop. Ecol. 19, 579–589.

  Darwin, C., 1897. The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. D. Appleton and Co., New

  York, 372 p.

  Dawkins, R., 1989. The Selfish Gene. Oxford University Press, New York (paperback edition, first

  publication 1976), 352 p.

  Dawkins, R., 2000. Unweaving the Rainbow. First Mariner Books (original publication 1998), 337 p.

  Dawkins, R., 2008. The Extended Phenotype. Oxford University Press, New York, (Original pub-

  lication 1982), 313 p.

  Dawkins, R., Krebs, J.R., 1978. Animal signals: information or manipulation? In: Krebs, J.R.,

  Davies, N.B. (Eds.), Behavioral Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach. Blackwell Scientific

  Publications, Oxford, pp. 282–309.

  de Waal, F.B.M., 1996. Good Natured: The Origins of Right and Wrong in Humans and Other Ani-

  mals. Harvard University Press, Cambridge.

  de Waal, F.B.M., 2008. Putting altruism back into altruism: the evolution of empathy. Annu. Rev.

  Psychol. 59, 279–300.

  de Waal, F.B.M., 2013. The Bonobo and the Atheist: In Search of Humanism Among the Primates.

  W.W. Norton and Co., New York, 289 p.

  Dieudonne, J., 1998. Keystone species as indicators of climate change. MS Thesis, University of

  Virginia, 147 p.

  Dixon, B.A., 2008. Animals, Emotion and Morality. Prometheus Books, New York, 281 p.

  Douglas-Hamilton, I., et al., 2006. Behavioral reactions of elephants towards a dying and deceased

  matriarch. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 100, 87–102.

  Eagleman, D.M., 2011. Incognito: The Secret Lives of the Brain. Pantheon Books, New York, 289 p.

  Evans, K.E., Harris, S., 2008. Adolescence in male African elephants, Loxodonta africana, and the

  importance of sociality. Anim. Behav. 76, 779–787.

  Fitch, W.T., 1997. Vocal tract length and formant frequency dispersion correlate with body size in

  rhesus macaques. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 102, 1213–1222.

  Fitch, W.T., 2000. Skull dimensions in relation to body size in nonhuman mammals: the causal

  bases for acoustic allometry. Zoology 103, 40–58.

  Fitch, W.T., Hauser, M.D., 2002. Unpacking “honesty” vertebrate vocal production and the evolu-

  tion of acoustic signals. In: Acoustic Communication, Simmons, A., Fay, R.R., Popper, N.

  References 121

  (Eds.), Springer Handbook of Auditory Research, vol. 16. Springer-Verlag, Berlin, Heidelberg,

  New York, p. 424.

  Fitch, W.T., Reby, D., 2001. The descended larynx is not uniquely human. P. Roy. Soc. Lond. B

  Bio. 268, 1669–1675.

  Foer, J., 2011. Moonwalking with Einstein: The Art and Science of Remembering Everything.

  Penguin Press, New York, 307 p.

  Foley, C., et al., 2008. Severe drought and calf survival in elephants. Biol. Lett. 4, 541–544.

  Franke, S.J., Swanson Jr., G.W., 1989. A brief tutorial on the fast field program (FFP) as applied to

  sound propagation in the air. Appl. Acoust. 27, 203–215.

  Garstang, M., 2004. Long-distance, low-frequency elephant communication. J. Comp. Physiol. A

  190, 791–805.

  Garstang, M., 2009. Precursor tsunami signals detected by elephants. The Open Conserve. Biol. J.

  3, 1–3.

  Garstang, M., Fitzjarrald, D., 1999. Observations of Surface to Atmosphere Interactions in the

  Tropics. Oxford University Press, New York, 405 p.

  Garstang, M., et al., 1995. Atmospheric controls on elephant communication. J. Exp. Biol. 198,

  939–951.

  Garstang, M., et al., 2005. The daily cycle of low-frequency elephant calls and near-surface

  atmospheric conditions. Earth Interact. 9, 1–21.

  Garstang, M., et al., 2014. Response of African elephants ( Loxodonta africana) to seasonal changes

  in rainfall. PLoS One 9, e108736, 13 p.

  Gould, J.L., 2004. Animal cognition. Curr. Biol. 14, 372–375.

  Gröning, K., Saller, M., 1999. Elephants: A Cultural and Natural History. Könemann Verlagsgesell-

  shaft
mbH, Cologne, 482 p. (English edition, original publication 1998).

  Gwynne, S.G., 2010. Empire of the Summer Moon. Scribner, a Division of Simon and Schuster,

  Inc., New York, p. 260.

  Hägstrum, J.T., 2000. Infrasound and the avian navigational map. J. Exp. Biol. 203, 1103–1111.

  Hamlin, J.K., Wynn, K., Bloom, P., 2007. Social evaluation by pre-verbal infants. Nature 450,

  557–560.

  Heffner, R., Heffner, H., 1982. Hearing in the elephant ( Elephas maximus): absolute sensitivity,

  frequency discrimination, and sound localization. J. Comp. Physiol. 96, 926–944.

  Heffner, R., Heffner, H., 1984. Sound localization in large mammals: localization of complex

  sounds by horses. Behav. Neurosci. 98, 541–555.

  Herbst, C.T., et al., 2013. Complex vibratory patterns in an elephant larynx. J. Exp. Biol. 216,

  4054–4064.

  Heyes, C.M., 1993. Anecdotes, training, trapping and triangulating: do animals attribute mental

  states? Anim. Behav. 46, 177–188.

  Holdrege, C., 2001. Elephantine intelligence. Nature Institute Monographs, Context #5 The Nature

  Institute, New York, 6 p.

  Hutto, J., 2006. Illumination in the Flatwoods: A Season Living Among the Wild Turkey, second ed.

  Lyons Press, Guilford, CT, paperback, 240 p.

  Hutto, J., 2014. Touching the Wild: Living with the Mule Deer of Deadman Gulch. Skyhorse Pub-

  lishing, New York, 306 p.

  Johanson, D., Edgar, B., 1996. From Lucy to Language. Wits University Press, Johannesburg, 272 p.

  Kelley, M., Garstang, M., 2013. On the possible detection of lightning storms by elephants. Animals

  3, 349–355.

  Krauss, R.M., 1987. The role of the listener: addressee influences on message formulation. J. Lang.

  Soc. Psychol. 6, 81–97.

  122 References

  Kuhn, G.F., 1977. Model for the interaural time differences in the azimuthal plane. J. Acoust. Soc.

  Am. 62, 157–167.

  Kuhn, G.F., 1987. Physical acoustic and measurements pertaining to directional hearing. In: Yost,

  W.A., Gourevitch, G. (Eds.), Directional Hearing. Academic Press, New York, pp. 305.

  Langbauer Jr., W.R., et al., 1991. African elephants respond to distant playback of low-frequency

  conspecific calls. J. Exp. Biol. 157, 35–46.

  Larom, D., et al., 1997. The influence of surface atmospheric conditions on the range and area

  reached by animal vocalizations. J. Exp. Biol. 200, 421–431.

  Lee, P.C., Moss, C.J., 2012. Wild female African elephants ( Loxodonta africana) exhibit personal-

 

‹ Prev