Young William James Thinking

Home > Other > Young William James Thinking > Page 12
Young William James Thinking Page 12

by Paul J Croce


  an im mensely thick sand deposit, . . . and reddish beds with only traces of

  stratification.” James dutifully reported the changes in the land, which could

  58  Young William James Thinking

  have been from the “first melting of the glacier,” but he showed reason to

  doubt the theory: evidence for “deposits of pres ent river mud” would have

  nothing to do with glaciers, and the “absence of fossils” meant limited evi-

  dence for any grand theory. Fourteen years after the trip to Brazil, James

  mentioned Agassiz as an example of a “ great man” who, like a Darwinian

  variation, no less, “brings about a rearrangement . . . of the pre- existing so-

  cial relations.” He even mocked Agassiz by equating his arrival in Amer i ca

  to the adaptive prowess of the Eu ro pean rabbit in New Zealand. James side-

  stepped the great man’s increasingly discredited theories in favor of

  praising his titanic character and his “concrete method of learning” and

  teaching.46

  During the expedition itself, James did not openly disagree with Agas-

  siz, but his Darwinian perspectives gradually emerged from careful obser-

  vations of nature. His teacher Wyman would have been proud. In a jungle

  habitat, James made an extended study of a certain active and curious spi-

  der monkey, whose species was “much the most in ter est ing of the South

  American quadrumana,” an early classification of monkeys and apes with

  four prehensile limbs. James’s account reflects the evolution of his scientific

  interests from biology to psy chol ogy: after a detailed account of the mon-

  key’s gross anatomy, he paid more attention to its be hav ior and emotions. At

  one point, this “best friend, was paying a little too much attention” to the

  sympathetic naturalist, and so, “I wd. forcibly tear my self away.” This pro-

  duced a “tragic- comic per for mance of despair.” He was tacitly supporting

  Darwinian propositions for continuity of species, even in mental and emo-

  tional life, and went on to describe not only the monkey’s physical agility

  but also its emotional range. “I am convinced,” he concluded, “that the vul-

  gar idea of a monkey being a merely ludicrous creature is all wrong.” With

  all his thorough description of his fellow primate’s antics, James was most

  interested in the way the monkey “suddenly stop[ped when] his attention

  [was] called to something else.” Recalling his medical interest in psychiatry,

  he reported that monkeys show an “utter inability to control their attention

  or their emotions, so that they are as completely possessed as insane people”;

  their attention follows “what ever feeling happens to be uppermost in them

  at the time,” and this gives them, James added, “a helplessness . . . which

  always recommends them to my pity.” Here James drew upon the prevail-

  ing hierarchical view of mind, ranging from adult humans with reasoning

  power to children, the insane, and animals with instincts; and he was tap-

  ping the accompanying fear of dissolution when the higher could no longer

  First Embrace of Science  59

  control the lower; for example, George Beard, who developed the diagnosis

  for “neurasthenia” that James would later apply to himself, said of insanity

  that it “makes us children, . . . makes us animals.” James’s informal jungle

  assessments provided an experiential prelude to his later theories about the

  power of focused and selective consciousness, which gives the reasoning

  primate an extra adaptive power.47 Louis Agassiz could appreciate James’s

  careful observations, but he would not countenance these psychological ap-

  plications of Darwinism.

  A major feature of Agassiz’s anti- Darwinian mission was his eagerness to

  promote his racist belief in the separate creation of the human races, with

  amalgamation creating degeneration: God had created human races sepa-

  rately, and humanity should maintain this segregation. Agassiz ordered

  Brazilians of vari ous complexions to pose for his gallery of racial hierarchy.

  James looked on in disbelief while vari ous “mocas [slang for Brazilians of

  mixed race] . . . were induced to strip and pose naked” for maximum ana-

  tomical analy sis. James had a completely diff er ent impression. He found

  the Brazilians “very nicely dressed in white muslin and jewelry, . . . appar-

  ently refined.” Describing “4 Indians” he had run into one day, he called one

  “respectable [and] civilized but still a perfect indian”; and he called “2 others

  excellent stout fellows and perfect gentlemen.” Where Agassiz searched for

  their faults, James asked, “[What] makes these people so refined and well

  bred?” In accord with Darwin, James pointed not only to continuities among

  primates but also to commonalities across races within humanity. James so

  thoroughly rejected Agassiz’s typologies and fear of miscegenation that,

  just a few years after the trip, he supported the idea that “humanity as a

  whole will have advanced” from “the mixture of races . . . taking place so

  inevitably all over the world.” 48 James was progressive not only in his think-

  ing about science and religion but also in his racial views.

  Sharply denying his teacher’s crude moral hierarchies, James noted that

  the people in front of him were, “at all events not sluttish.” He sounds like

  Huck Finn feeling guilty for disagreeing with the racism surrounding

  him; James maintained that even though “ these are peasants,” scorned

  by Brazilian society and by his own expedition leader, “no gentleman of

  Eu rope has better manners.” And he added that the Brazilians, “masters

  and servants” alike, had “not a bit of our damned anglo saxon brutality and

  vulgarity.” James even found that the people so readily regarded by most

  Eu ro pean Americans as exotic primitives lived lives not unlike what he knew

  back home. He recognized that “the amazonians have not the pleasures of

  60  Young William James Thinking

  [the] domestic hearth which are so dear to us, . . . but in the mosquito net,

  hardly domestic, but personal they have a faint substitute for it.” While in

  their house hold, you have the “feeling of . . . security” that you get with a

  “big blazing fireside in our winters [when] you hear the icy storm at work

  out of doors.” By contrast, to support his theory of racial hierarchy, Agassiz

  regarded Brazil as a case study of his worst social fears since “all clearness

  of type had been blurred, . . . leaving a mongrel nondescript type.” Yet even

  Agassiz and his wife had to admire a “cafuzo” (with “a mixture of Indian

  and black blood in her veins”), who worked for Elizabeth Agassiz. Young

  Alexandrina had “keen perceptions” and was “a very efficient aid.” They

  were, however, very ready to explain away her virtues by adding dismis-

  sively that she was “a person whose only training has been through the

  senses”; their conclusion resembled the patronizing racial theories of Henry

  James, Se nior. Yet William’s view bore ironic resemblance to another of his

  father’s theories, about the best way to raise his own very non- cafuzo

  chil
dren: with a sensuous education. In sharp contrast with Agassiz’s ex-

  ploitative portraits, young James sketched Alexandrina with individual

  dignity, a hint of sadness, and even skeptical detachment; and her serene

  look and knowing eyes suggest that her spontaneous encounters with nature,

  as with the ancients he admired, would give her insights into nature that the

  civilized scientists overlooked— just as young James had growing doubts

  about scientific naturalism. Here was a very tangible version of a young prag-

  matist finding intellectual merit and insight among indigenous people, out-

  looks phi los o pher Scott Pratt has identified as “native pragmatism. ”49

  Trying on Natu ral History

  In addition to mea sur ing out his differences with Agassiz, James also con-

  tinued puzzling about his career choices. On the day before he set sail to

  Brazil, he wondered if Agassiz would give him a clue “about my fate.” James

  earnestly resolved that the trip would be a voyage of self- discovery: “I said

  to myself before I came away: ‘W.J., in this excursion you will learn to know

  yourself and your resources somewhat more intimately than you do now,

  and will come back with your character considerably evolved and estab-

  lished.’ ” While he could not yet define a par tic u lar career, he was looking

  for some vocational direction, at least by discovering what he did not want

  to do. He found out that most of the work would be “mechanical, finding

  objects & packing them,” with little time for “studying their structure.” To

  make matters worse, in early May James developed a mild form of small-

  William James’s Huck Finn Moment.

  Agassiz and Agassiz, A Journey in Brazil (1868), 245, 246, and xviii.

  Louis and Elizabeth Cary Agassiz included a drawing by William James of one

  their local helpers, only identified as Alexandrina, in the account they wrote about

  their natu ral history expedition to Brazil in 1865–66. Despite Louis Agassiz’s

  eagerness to show that the nonwhites of Brazil were inferior to Eu ro pe ans, he relied

  on many of these “inferior” people as vital resources for collecting local samples, and

  young James drew a portrait of Alexandrina, one of their most reliable assistants. In

  their account, the Agassizes treat her hair as an “extraordinary” natu ral find and

  assured their readers that James’s drawing “is in no way exaggerated.” In fact, her

  “wiry elasticity . . . stood six inches beyond the shoulders each way.” Their constant

  search for racial inferiorities in Brazil, culminating with the image of Alexandrina

  looking “as if electrified,” prompted Louis Menand’s elegant ridicule, “They found

  hierarchy in hair” ( Metaphysical Club, 136). By contrast, in his portrait, James clearly

  displays the native woman’s dignity and intelligence, with even some hints of sadness

  or anger. It was a Huck Finn moment with James wordlessly defying his teacher’s

  racist assumptions and Alexandrina’s knowing eyes defying the words surrounding

  her in the Agassiz book.

  62  Young William James Thinking

  pox, which dampened what ever enthusiasm flourished at the start. Although

  he would feel better by mid- June, the disease gave him “red tubercles,” which

  left his face with “the appearance of an im mense ripe raspberry,” and an

  ongoing sensitivity in his eyes. He thought of his attending doctor as “a per-

  fect brick,” which did not raise his confidence about the diagnosis or about

  mainstream medicine in general. In the near term, however, the most direct

  impact was on his mood. He wrote glumly to his family that “my coming

  was a mistake. ”50

  James’s mood and health mingled with his vocational questions, as the

  field work convinced him that “I am cut out for a speculative rather than an

  active life.” Remembering his discussions at home, especially with Charles

  Peirce and other friends, and his wide reading back in Cambridge, he even

  specified, for the first time in so many words, that “when I get home I’m

  going to study philosophy all my days.” And he also recalled his 1858 worry

  about “working in the wrong direction”: he now pledged to avoid work

  that was “not in my path and was so much waste of life.” Still, he realized

  that some edification could come in a hard- won way, even “through some

  great mistake”; prob lems, after all, could be learning opportunities— learning

  “through . . . individual experience.” 51 He would have plenty more opportu-

  nities to learn from his trou bles in the next few years, and experience would

  continue to be a teacher to his own development and his philosophical

  speculations.

  James’s feelings about the trip also sharpened his vocational thinking: “I

  shall learn next to nothing of natu ral history as I care about learning it.”

  Like his father, James had high hopes with the study of nature, but in Bra-

  zil, by contrast, “the affair reduces itself thus to so many months spent in

  physical exercise.” He vented his frustrations with Agassiz’s directives to

  collect Amazonian creatures in encyclopedic abundance by mockingly pro-

  claiming that they had gathered “4,00000000000 new species [of] fish.” He

  portrayed this ballooned number, 40 trillion, on a banner in a cartoon sketch

  that also included “new and hitherto unknown genera of animals,” each one

  specially created. James was spoofing Agassiz’s tendency to “split” or group

  subtle variations into distinct species in contrast with Darwinians, who gen-

  erally regarded diff er ent individuals as va ri e ties within species, and thus as

  indicators of stages in evolution. Despite his disagreements with Agassiz,

  James was still interested in natu ral science, but he wanted more reflec-

  tions on methods and implications rather than the “mechanical . . . work” of

  First Embrace of Science  63

  a field naturalist.52 It was just these reflections on methods that would be-

  come impor tant for his philosophy.

  Despite his prob lems with day- to- day natu ral history, James stayed with

  the expedition, and after his recovery from smallpox, he hoped that “I shall

  have materially a better time.” In addition to working regularly in the field,

  he wrote notes and letters back home full of reports about Brazilian society,

  the luxuriant Amazonian landscape, and the “several billion mosquitoes

  and flies” that followed them everywhere in the tropics. For example, he

  observed that despite the “fertility of the country,” the residents often lived

  without a “sufficient stock of food.” And yet, they poured out generosity; at

  a missionary settlement, the “ mother cooked our dinners,” and he comple-

  mented the “handsome family of nearly white god children,” so called, he

  added slyly, “from courtesy to the cloth.” By the last months, he was genuinely

  enjoying the trip, both because he was “ doing and gaining so much” practical

  scientific study and because it was a genuinely exotic adventure. Still, for all

  his excitement about the landscape and culture, he largely remained a specta-

  tor. In his frustration, he would grow depressed, such as when he opened

 
; one letter with the words, “Woe is me.” But he did not dwell there, going on

  to explain “I have gone through a good deal of blueness so far produced by

  my sickness.” Once feeling better, he was “beginning to see that the voyage

  has been an excellent thing for me & [I] enjoy it more and more every

  day.” His mood also received a boost after meeting a group of Spanish ex-

  plorers; James said, “[N]ever had I seen a more shaggy, stained, weather-

  beaten, jaundiced set of men. . . . Besides their travels our expedition seemed

  like a holiday picnic.” Through his ups and downs, he kept writing letters

  and taking notes of his observations, and he even scolded himself for falling

  “ behind . . . 4 days” in his work. Still, he was convinced that “collecting [is

  not] suited to my genius at all,” but even that insight made the “exercise . . .

  better for me.” 53 Through the drudgery, he was gaining an intimate feel for

  the practice and methods of scientific research and, more broadly, was com-

  ing to appreciate the constructive aspects of even negative experiences.

  Scientific research and reflective speculation were not the only fields that

  beckoned on this expedition; lush and colorful jungle settings reminded

  him of his earlier interest in painting. He noted the beauty around him with

  a paint er’s eye and an artist’s sense of the limits of verbal expression— quite

  literally, as he blurted out, “No words, but only savage inarticulate cries,

  can express the gorgeous loveliness of the walk I have been taking. Houp

  A Staggering Number of New Species in Brazil.

  [The Winding Caravan of New Species in Brazil], William James papers, bMS Am

  1092.9 (4498). Courtesy of Bay James and Houghton Library, Harvard University.

  During his travel with Louis Agassiz’s natu ral history expedition to Brazil in

  1865–66, William James not only gained a deep appreciation for the importance of

  fact gathering but also spoofed his teacher’s anti- Darwinian tendency to identify

  new species in every new variety and in each distinct landscape. By contrast,

  Charles Darwin, in his theory of species development, first publicly explained in 1859,

  depicted va ri e ties as stages in evolutionary development. In this sketch, James

 

‹ Prev