Women Don't Ask

Home > Other > Women Don't Ask > Page 23
Women Don't Ask Page 23

by Linda Babcock


  information and other externally set guidelines to set goals can improve

  women’s negotiation results substantially.20 Rhonda is the manager of a

  very successful mutual fund. After running this fund for almost ten

  years, she concluded that she was being grossly underpaid. But when

  she asked her boss for a salary increase, he turned her down. Rhonda

  responded by researching what other top fund managers earned. When

  she discovered that very few women had achieved her level of success,

  she contacted several top male managers and asked what they were

  paid. She then went back to her boss armed with this information. This

  time, he granted her request. Now she’s the highest-paid fund manager

  at the company.

  Another one of Linda’s studies with Hannah Riley and Kathleen

  McGinn suggests that the use of external guidelines not only increases

  women’s goals and helps them achieve better negotiated settlements—

  it may even eliminate the gender gap in outcomes. For this study, they

  recruited undergraduates from the Boston area to participate in negotia-

  tions and divided them into two groups. They gave one group a bottom

  line—for sellers the minimum they could accept and for buyers the

  maximum they could pay. They gave the other group both a bottom

  line and a goal—an amount they were to shoot for in the negotiation.

  Among the students who received just a bottom line (the bottom lines

  given to men and women were identical), female buyers set less aggres-

  sive goals than male buyers (10 percent less) and negotiated prices that

  137

  C H A P T E R 6

  were 27 percent worse than those achieved by the male buyers. But

  among those students who were provided with both bottom lines and

  goals, there was no gender difference in negotiated prices.21 In other

  words, gender differences in outcomes may be eliminated if male and

  female negotiators are working toward identical goals.

  This has two important implications. First, it points out the critical

  role played by goals in causing gender differences. Second, it shines a

  light on one way in which women can improve the results of their

  negotiations—by spending more time researching aggressive yet poten-

  tially obtainable goals before they begin.

  The Rewards of Risk

  If optimism is about how much we believe is available, risk-taking is

  about “going for it”—taking a chance to get as much as we can—and

  men seem to be more comfortable than women taking risks. Studies

  that have included subjects from all over the world have shown that

  women are less likely than men to engage in activities that involve phys-

  ical risk.22 Men also score higher on “sensation-seeking” scales. These

  scales (actually four subscales), developed by the psychologist Marvin

  Zuckerman, measure: “thrill and adventure seeking” (a desire to partici-

  pate in physical risk-taking activities); “experience seeking” (wanting

  to pursue new and different experiences); “disinhibition” (interest in

  pursuing hedonistic pleasure); and “boredom susceptibility” (a dislike

  of dull and boring people, activities, or environments).23 Men score

  higher than women in each category.

  Men and women also approach activities involving social risk differ-

  ently. Males are “more likely to see a challenge that calls forth participa-

  tion” in a socially risky situation, according to the psychologist Eliza-

  beth Arch, whereas females more commonly perceive such activities as

  threatening and try to avoid them.24 Because negotiation by definition

  contains the possibility of rejection or failure, it always involves a certain amount of social risk—making it potentially threatening to women.

  Women’s fear that negotiating for what they want can damage their

  relationships may also make negotiation seem socially risky to women.

  Arch argues that women’s fear of taking social risks prompts them

  to behave more cautiously than men. If this is true, their greater sense

  of caution may prevent girls and women from breaking rules and chal-

  lenging the status quo while they’re growing up. As a result, they may

  138

  L O W G O A L S A N D S A F E T A R G E T S

  not discover that this kind of risk taking—and by extension any unsanc-

  tioned approach to getting what they want (such as asking for some-

  thing when it hasn’t been offered)—can be a successful strategy. Men’s

  greater propensity to take risks may teach them, in contrast, that chal-

  lenging the status quo and pushing their own agendas can work to their

  advantage. Many women learn this lesson much more slowly, if at all.

  One relatively new line of research suggests that men’s biology may

  actually prime them to feel more comfortable taking risks. Several re-

  cent studies have shown a correlation between testosterone levels in

  men and dominance behaviors, for example. Dominance behaviors are

  defined as actions designed to improve a person’s power, status, and

  access to desirable rewards (material or otherwise).25 Men who receive

  testosterone injections to treat a variety of medical conditions report

  feeling more confident, aggressive, and ready to take risks immediately

  after their treatment and for several days afterward—until their testos-

  terone levels sink again.26 Testosterone levels in male athletes have also

  been shown to rise before a competition,27 with some researchers sug-

  gesting that this upsurge may increase an athlete’s readiness to take

  risks,28 and others speculating that these elevated testosterone levels

  may improve coordination, enhance mental functioning, and aid con-

  centration.29 Studies have also shown that testosterone levels remain

  elevated among “winners” but fall in “losers.”30 This suggests that when

  risk taking is rewarded by success, men experience a biological reac-

  tion—a sustained testosterone “high,” if you will—that promotes more

  risk taking. Another study shows that the testosterone levels of men in

  occupations that involve a lot of personal risk, such as actors, profes-

  sional football players, and firefighters, tend to be higher than those of

  men in professions that involve less personal risk, such as ministers

  and doctors.31 Women also have testosterone in their systems (in much

  smaller amounts), but researchers have not yet demonstrated any con-

  sistent correlation between changes in women’s testosterone levels and

  their behavior.32

  Although a great deal of controversy surrounds this area of study, and

  it’s important to remember that “correlation does not imply causation”

  (there’s no proof yet that rises in testosterone under certain circum-

  stances actually cause the behaviors observed), the link to negotiation is not hard to make. If the prospect of competition that is embedded in

  any negotiation raises a man’s testosterone level, that rise in testosterone

  may help him feel more confident about setting higher targets for the

  negotiation and making a more aggressive first offer—taking greater

  139

  C H A P T E R 6

  risks in order to get as much as h
e can. Once the negotiation has begun,

  if a man is negotiating effectively, each perceived “win” in the interac-

  tion may serve to keep his testosterone elevated, thus maintaining his

  optimistic outlook, improving his cognitive functioning and concentra-

  tion, and helping him continue to pursue a more aggressive target settle-

  ment. A woman in the same situation, unaided by this biological “facili-

  tator of risk,”33 may not only set a less aggressive target in the first place, she may also concede more rapidly if she fears that pushing for what

  she wants is socially risky. Or she may concede more because the man

  she’s negotiating with, urged on by the testosterone in his system, con-

  tinues pushing for his goal much longer.

  Just as excessive optimism can be problematic for men, however,

  “testosterone-driven impatience may lead to poor decision-making,”34

  which can leave men with bad agreements, with no agreements, or with

  relationship problems created by their overly aggressive behavior.

  The Confidence Gap

  Women also set lower targets and settle for less in their negotiations

  because they lack confidence in their ability to negotiate effectively.35

  Assuming that they’re no good at negotiating, they conclude that they

  won’t be able to attain higher goals.

  Psychologists have demonstrated that the more self-confident people

  feel about a particular task, the more likely they are to set high goals

  for that task and persist in trying to achieve those goals. More self-

  confident people stay in the game, that is, trying to find ways to get

  what they want; less-confident people give up sooner.36 Lindsey, 41, a

  research chemist, although quite successful, conforms to this pattern:

  I get so nervous in negotiating that I capitulate very quickly. So, for

  instance . . . when I took my previous job, I felt as though for form’s

  sake . . . you shouldn’t take what you’re offered, you should always

  ask for a little more. So . . . I go in there and say it, and the person

  to whom I say it sits back and says, ‘Well, what do you mean by that?’

  Or, ‘Are you saying blah, blah, blah?’ And then I find myself going,

  ‘Oh, never mind, I didn’t really mean it. I’m happy to have the job,’

  or whatever. And I just capitulate so quickly after just making a show

  of trying to get something, because . . . I’m not very good at profes-

  sional negotiating.

  140

  L O W G O A L S A N D S A F E T A R G E T S

  Lindsey’s lack of self-confidence prevents her from “sticking to her

  guns” and holding out for what she wants.

  In addition to limiting the goals women set for themselves, their lack

  of self-confidence can undermine women’s efforts in another way. A

  1983 study by the psychologists Debra Instone, Brenda Major, and Bar-

  bara Bunker examined the interrelationships among self-confidence, in-

  fluence attempts, and gender. They divided students into groups of four

  and assigned one student in each group to supervise the other three

  while they performed a set task. During this exercise, the researchers

  observed the extent to which the “supervisors” attempted to influence

  the productivity of their “workers” and the types of influence strategies

  they used. Separately, the researchers also measured the supervisors’

  self-confidence about their managerial ability using a simple five-point

  scale. They found that the male “supervisors” felt more confidence in

  their managerial ability than the women (the men’s average scores were

  31 percent higher than the women’s)—and that the male “supervisors”

  made more attempts than the female supervisors to influence the pro-

  ductivity of their “workers.” Even more to the point, the higher the

  supervisors scored on the self-confidence scale, the more frequently

  they tried to influence the productivity of their workers. Had the male

  and female supervisors had similar levels of self-confidence about their

  managerial ability, this suggests, there would have been no difference

  in the frequency of their influence attempts.37

  How does this relate to negotiation? As we’ve already noted, negotia-

  tion is essentially a mutual influence-attempt process—it’s all about try-

  ing to influence another person or group of people to do something or

  give you something you want. Since women as a group feel less self-

  confident about their negotiating abilities, they not only attempt fewer

  negotiations, they also try to influence the decision of the opposite ne-

  gotiator fewer times: Their lack of confidence impels them to try only

  once or twice to get what they want before conceding.

  Lana, a trauma surgeon at a New York hospital and a friend of Lin-

  da’s, applied for parental leave before the birth of her first child. The

  hospital’s policy was to give new mothers six weeks of pregnancy

  leave and then four additional months during which they were exempt

  from working night and weekend shifts. But when Lana asked her de-

  partment head for the leave, he said they were short-staffed and she

  could only have two months off from night and weekend call. Having

  asked once and been turned down, Lana wasn’t going to try again. She

  thought she had no choice but to take the two months he’d offered and

  141

  C H A P T E R 6

  then go back to her usual grueling schedule. Fortunately, she men-

  tioned what had happened to Linda, and Linda convinced her that she

  was entitled to the full benefits stipulated by hospital policy. Lana went

  back to her department head, tried again, and this time persuaded him

  to give her the full four months off. Like many other women, Lana’s

  lack of self-confidence, particularly around negotiating, had led her to

  concede too soon.

  Interestingly, however, the psychologist Ellen Lenney has found that

  gender differences in self-confidence depend upon context and situa-

  tion, with women’s perceptions of their ability more context-dependent

  than men’s. This means that women’s feelings of self-confidence fluctu-

  ate more than men’s in response to the specifics of a situation.38 Follow-

  ing up on this research, the psychologists Sylvia Beyer and Edward

  Bowden found that women tend to feel more self-confident about cer-

  tain types of activities than others—and especially lack confidence

  about activities that are strongly identified with men.39 Some researchers

  believe that negotiation, particularly on one’s own behalf, is one of those

  activities.40 This makes a lot of sense to us, since negotiation falls more

  in line with social expectations for male behavior (being self-promoting

  and aggressive) than with those for female behavior (being other-di-

  rected and selfless). Not surprisingly, this is what Linda and her col-

  leagues found in their web survey—that people’s estimations of their

  own ability as negotiators differed noticeably by gender, with men rat-

  ing their ability higher on average than women.41

  Increasing Control

  Is there hope? Are there ways for women to improve their self-confi-

  dence around
negotiating—and achieve better outcomes? Although

  very little research has been done on training or tactics that will improve

  women’s negotiating results, one study offers some useful clues. After

  demonstrating distinct gender differences in negotiated outcomes

  among a group of MBA students, three professors of management, Cyn-

  thia Kay Stevens, Anna Bavetta, and Marilyn Gist, explored whether

  these differences could be eliminated by different types of training inter-

  ventions.42 In the first stage of the study, the researchers gave a group

  of students four hours of classroom training in useful tactics for salary

  negotiations. Then they asked each student to negotiate a salary for a

  hypothetical job. Confederates of the researchers played the role of a

  142

  L O W G O A L S A N D S A F E T A R G E T S

  personnel director at a firm that wanted to hire the students, and all

  the confederates used identical “scripts.” This was called the “baseline”

  negotiation, and the researchers found that gender differences in goals

  (men’s were higher) closely paralleled the gender gap in negotiated re-

  sults (the men negotiated better salaries).

  After this negotiation, the student subjects attended two more hours

  of training, but this time they were separated into two groups. One

  group attended a session about goal-setting while the other attended a

  session about “self-management” techniques. The “goals” session

  showed how the use of challenging goals can improve performance and

  described ways to set appropriate goals for salary negotiations. The in-

  structors did not tell the students what goals to set, however. They

  merely taught them how to set aggressive goals and emphasized the

  importance of doing so. In the other training session, students were

  taught five “self-management” principles. These included anticipating

  performance obstacles by identifying situations that might cause them

  anxiety or stress and planning to overcome those performance obstacles

  by developing strategies to deal with anxiety-producing situations. They

  also included practicing their responses with a partner to build their

  self-confidence. In addition, students were encouraged to set perfor-

  mance goals by evaluating all potential outcomes and ranking their pri-

  orities, identifying “giveaways,” and settling on targets as well as “re-

 

‹ Prev