The Best American Science and Nature Writing 2020

Home > Science > The Best American Science and Nature Writing 2020 > Page 43
The Best American Science and Nature Writing 2020 Page 43

by Michio Kaku


  * * *

  The first time Collins collapsed because of a seizure, he was four and being minded by a babysitter. Over time, his symptoms grew more varied and more severe. “It’s like my brain froze,” he says. “I was really confused, and then I’d get really nauseous, throw up, and then I’d be kind of acting normal again.”

  A daily ritual ensued: Collins would go to school, have a seizure, collapse, and go home. Still, despite the misery, the seizures were a blessing in disguise. They led to the discovery of the tumor slowly enveloping a piece of his brain.

  “These are some of the most common tumors we see in children,” says Christina Patterson, MD, a pediatric epilepsy neurologist and part of the medical team that prepared Collins for surgery at the UPMC Children’s Hospital of Pittsburgh. “Taking out the tumor is ultimately the cure.”

  The deeper problem with pediatric tumors like the one Collins developed—​beyond the nausea, headaches, and confusion that he experienced—​is that the seizures they produce can damage the electrical networks of the brain.

  “We know that the pediatric brain has plasticity, [and] that we’re constantly creating new algorithms in the brain to live life,” Patterson says. “But when you have seizures on top of that, those disrupted electrical networks that are the seizures prevent any kind of meaningful remapping.”

  Inside our brains are about 100 billion neurons. These neurons build thousands of connections with one another and communicate with their cellular brethren by converting electrical signals into chemical neurotransmitters, which are responsible for carrying information between the brain cells. As we master new skills, the brain’s neurons form new connections and strengthen old ones that aided in learning that information. Instead of discrete regions carrying out specific tasks, the brain depends on groups of neural networks talking to each other across multiple regions. (Behrmann says a single neuron can communicate with 50,000 other cells.) If the network is damaged, the brain cells can’t communicate effectively.

  Picture a map of the United States that shows a phone company’s LTE network crisscrossing the country, and you have a rough approximation of how the human brain operates. Surgery for Collins, in this case, was akin to repairing a downed cell tower.

  Before Collins’s surgery to remove the tumor, doctors opened up his head and placed electrodes on the surface of his brain and inside his visual cortex. For seven days, Collins lay in a hospital bed as the electrodes mapped his brain’s electrical activity, creating what was essentially a schematic diagram showing doctors where the seizures were originating and which brain areas needed to be cut out.

  Collins recognized his parents after the surgery, but he couldn’t match their faces to their names. The problem resolved itself in a couple of days, but the episode left Nicole and Carl concerned: how was their son’s brain going to function with a missing part?

  * * *

  Consider, for a moment, a page from a Where’s Waldo? book. When your eye focuses on the crowded image, you’re actually only receiving two types of feedback: the light that falls on the retina and the color of that light. “That’s all your eye can pick up,” Behrmann says. “Yet somehow, almost instantaneously, you get an interpretation of the scene.”

  Patterson put the Collins family in touch with Behrmann, who studies how brain plasticity relates to vision at her lab at Carnegie Mellon. Collins was the ideal candidate for Behrmann’s research. Children’s brains are young and still developing and therefore have the most potential for neuroplastic change. Because Collins’s tumor formed in the part of the brain crucial for visual processing, Behrmann could track his progress over time to determine whether there were any lingering deficits in his ability to interpret images. Because Collins was a child, his brain was also in a critical period of development where it builds the capacity to recognize faces, something that happens gradually and becomes more finely tuned throughout our teenage years.

  As University of Toronto psychiatrist Norman Doidge notes in his 2007 book, The Brain That Changes Itself, the notion that there is a critical period of brain development is one of the most important discoveries in the area of neuroplasticity—​and one for which we have kittens to thank. In the 1960s, as Doidge recounts, scientists David Hubel and Torsten Wiesel mapped the visual cortex of kittens—​much in the same way Collins’s surgical team mapped his own brain—​to learn how vision is processed. Then, in an admittedly grisly procedure, the scientists sewed shut the eyelid of one of the kittens in the study. Upon opening the eyelid, they found that the visual areas of the kitten’s brain responsible for processing images from that eye didn’t develop, leaving the kitten blind in that eye, even though nothing was biologically wrong with the eye. The researchers discovered that if kittens’ brains were to develop normally, they had to be able to see the world around them between their third and eighth weeks of life.

  But another discovery from the study proved even more important—​and earned Hubel and Wiesel the Nobel Prize. “The part of the kitten’s brain that had been deprived of input from the shut eye did not remain idle,” Doidge writes. “It had begun to process visual input from the open eye, as though the brain didn’t want to waste any ‘cortical real estate’ and had found a way to rewire itself.”

  In Collins’s case, the question was whether the fully intact left hemisphere of his brain would pick up the functionality of the missing third of his brain, especially the task of facial recognition, which is typically carried out by the right hemisphere.

  Starting just before Collins was seven and continuing for three years, Behrmann administered a series of tests roughly every six months. In one challenge, he was shown photos of faces in intervals of roughly thirty seconds. If he remembered a face, he clicked a button. A similar test was administered using photos of houses, and if Collins saw the same photo back to back, he clicked a button. Each test occurred while he was inside a functional MRI machine, which allowed Behrmann to measure the flow of blood and oxygen to different regions of the brain. The more active an area of the brain, the more blood it draws.

  Throughout these experiments, Behrmann compared Collins’s brain function to a control group of kids his own age without brain abnormalities. The results, published last August in Cell Reports, were striking: his neurological function was “absolutely normal,” with no subtle delays or deviations in development.

  Over coffee in the kitchen of her Pittsburgh home, Behrmann showed me successive scans of Collins’s brain that told the tale. “When he was eight, you can see the first glimmerings of face recognition in the brain,” she says. “By the time he got to ten, you can see that his left hemisphere looks really like the right hemisphere of the controls.”

  In scans, Collins’s left brain not only looked and performed the way his left brain should; it also looked similar in scans as other kids’ intact right brains. That’s because the functions of the visual cortex he lost by having one-third of his right brain removed—​the ability to see objects and know what they are, and the ability to recognize faces—​were subsumed by his left brain. Also fascinating to Behrmann was how the left brain could accommodate two different skills: word recognition, which is the domain of the left brain, as well as facial recognition. Indeed, part of the surprise was that the left brain could keep doing what it normally does in addition to the newly added right-brain activity.

  In other words, Behrmann’s work revealed that Collins’s brain rewired itself, like the brain of the kitten that Hubel and Wiesel studied.

  Just how the brain accomplishes this feat remains a central question. By analyzing brain scans using a neuroimaging technique known as diffusion tensor imaging, which shows how water travels along the brain’s white-matter tracts, Behrmann has found initial glimmerings that the white matter of the brain—​the electrical wiring that underlines communication between multiple neurological regions—​actually changes. Areas of the brain that weren’t connected before create new links, an example of neuroplasticity in a
ction that may preserve brain functionality. But scientists still don’t know what triggers the cells of the white matter to behave in this way.

  “When Tanner is twenty, I think we’ll know a lot more about the overall wiring,” Behrmann says. “The one thing that we will not know in humans, and I don’t know how we will ever know it, are the changes that occur at the level of the cells themselves.”

  * * *

  Every three to six months, Collins returns to Behrmann’s lab to undergo tests and be examined for any visual deficits. Behrmann hopes that following him over time will lead to more definitive answers, not only about how his visual system finally reorganizes itself but also the process by which it does so. “We’ve got a long way to go, but the work, I think, is really exciting,” she says.

  In a follow-up study Behrmann conducted with Collins and nine other children—​all of whom are missing areas of either their left or right hemisphere—​eight of them, including Collins, showed absolutely normal vision function. The two who did not are children whose brain damage from seizures was more severe prior to their surgeries.

  This sort of insight is needed to gauge when to perform a brain surgery like the one Collins had. At what age should parents agree to remove a tumor that’s causing epileptic seizures? Sometimes, resective surgery that removes brain tissue can make it difficult for a person to use and understand words; it can also, as it did in Collins’s case, result in visual impairment.

  “Once we have a better picture of exactly what happens after we remove large segments of the brain, we may be able to counsel families more effectively,” says Taylor Abel, MD, a pediatric neurosurgeon who specializes in epilepsy surgery and arrived at the Children’s Hospital of Pittsburgh last summer to begin collaborating with Behrmann. “The goal should be to do whatever you can to stop the seizures and get off of medications as early in your life as possible. The sooner you do that, the sooner you can return to a normal developmental trajectory.”

  It may even be the case, Abel and Behrmann point out, that some of the reorganization that took place in Collins’s brain started prior to his scheduled surgery. It’s not something Behrmann can prove, since all the research conducted on Collins has taken place post-surgery.

  “When you have an abnormality in your brain that’s causing seizures, that abnormality can actually cause the brain to reorganize or start reorganizing before the surgery actually takes place,” Abel says. “But the other thing that sometimes happens is that the seizures affect the functions in the brain, and the brain doesn’t reorganize.”

  Behrmann says one of the fundamental goals of her research is to study a large enough population of children to determine if there are patterns of optimal recovery based on the age they had their surgery. Reorganization to the degree Collins has experienced is impossible for adults undergoing similar surgery, Behrmann says, as they lack the neuroplasticity seen in children.

  For Nicole and Carl, the surgery was unequivocally the right decision. “What was happening before the surgery was pretty awful,” Nicole says. “After surgery, the changes were only for the better. Yeah, he has his visual deficits. But everything else was for the better.”

  In late 2017, a follow-up MRI at the Children’s Hospital of Pittsburgh showed that Collins’s tumor grew back. This time, though, it was the size of a pea. Two months later, in February 2018, surgeons opened his brain a second time. Collins says the prospect of a second surgery didn’t bother him; he just wanted the pea-size tumor out of his head so he wouldn’t have to worry about it. (The surgery went well, and he’s still tumor-free.)

  * * *

  As we close in on minute 24 of our chess match, I move my king in the corner of the board, still certain of my impending doom. Collins scans his remaining white pieces and then takes a look at where his king sits.

  “Mate,” he says, looking up at me.

  Checkmate for me, I realize, surprised by a victory I did not expect. Collins begins breaking down the moves he made, retracing some of his steps. It seems he forgot about a pawn of mine that was still on the board.

  “I like losing,” he says. “Obviously, I like winning too. But when you lose, you gain the knowledge.”

  Even after losing a portion of his brain, Collins is still learning. His brain is still growing, still adapting—​and, even if it’s not readily apparent, still changing.

  Contributors’ Notes

  Ross Andersen is deputy editor of The Atlantic. He was previously deputy editor of Aeon Magazine.

  Kelly Clancy is a writer and neuroscientist at University College London.

  Daniel Duane is the author of Caught Inside: A Surfer’s Year on the California Coast. He is at work on a memoir about the Sierra Nevada.

  David H. Freedman is a Boston-based science writer. He is a contributing writer for The Atlantic and the author of five books, the most recent of which is Wrong, about the problems with the published findings of scientists and other types of expertise.

  Rivka Galchen, a novelist and essayist, is the author of four books.

  Bahar Gholipour is a neuroscientist turned journalist covering biomedicine research, neuroscience, and AI for a wide variety of publications, including Scientific American, The Atlantic, the New York Times, Science, New York Magazine, NBC News, and the Daily Beast. She grew up in Tehran and lives in New York.

  Adam Gopnik has been a staff writer at The New Yorker since 1987. He has won the National Magazine Award for Essays and Criticism three times, and his books include At the Strangers’ Gate and A Thousand Small Sanities.

  Sara Harrison is a freelance journalist who covers science, business, and technology. She is a graduate of the UC Berkeley School of Journalism and Carleton College.

  Patrick House received a PhD in neuroscience from Stanford University. His nonfiction writing has appeared in The New Yorker online, Slate, and the Los Angeles Review of Books.

  Ferris Jabr is a contributing writer for the New York Times Magazine. He has also written for The New Yorker, Harper’s, The Atlantic, Lapham’s Quarterly, Wired, Outside, Scientific American, and Foreign Policy. He is currently working on his first book. He lives in Portland, Oregon.

  Sarah Kaplan is a Washington Post climate reporter covering humanity’s response to our warming world. She has reported from the sea ice off Alaska, hurricane-lashed communities in North Carolina, and massive protests on the National Mall, seeking stories of the people most affected by climate change as well as those working hardest to halt it. She previously worked on the Post’s science desk, where she wrote about space and the physical sciences.

  Adam Mann is a freelance space and physics reporter living in Oakland, California. His work has appeared in many places, including Wired, the New York Times, the Wall Street Journal, and Scientific American. In his spare time, he likes thinking about aliens and riding his bike.

  Deanna Csomo McCool is a science writer and assistant director of marketing communications in the College of Science at the University of Notre Dame, where she also teaches a course in science journalism. She graduated from Indiana University, earned her master’s degree in science writing twenty-five years later from Johns Hopkins University, and has written for a variety of publications. She lives a few miles from Notre Dame with her daughter and a “vicious” rescue dog—​a shih-tzu mix.

  Jon Mooallem is a writer at large for the New York Times Magazine and the author, most recently, of This Is Chance!, about the 1964 Great Alaska Earthquake and Anchorage radio reporter Genie Chance.

  Melinda Wenner Moyer is a science journalist based in New York’s Hudson Valley and a visiting scholar at NYU’s Arthur L. Carter Journalism Institute. She is a contributing editor at Scientific American magazine and a columnist at Slate magazine. She was the recipient of the 2019 Bricker Award for Science Writing in Medicine. Her work has also received first-place awards in the American Society of Journalists and Authors’ Annual Writing Awards and the Awards for Excellence in Health Care Reporting.

 
Siddhartha Mukherjee is the author of The Emperor of All Maladies, winner of the 2011 Pulitzer Prize in general nonfiction, and of The Laws of Medicine. He is the guest editor of The Best American Science and Nature Writing 2013. His latest work, The Gene, recounts the quest to decipher the master code of instructions that makes and defines humans and determines the future of our children. Mukherjee is an assistant professor of medicine at Columbia University and a cancer physician and researcher. A Rhodes scholar, he graduated from Stanford University, the University of Oxford, and Harvard Medical School. He has published articles in Nature, The New Yorker, the New England Journal of Medicine, the New York Times, and Cell. He lives in New York with his wife and daughters.

  Douglas Preston is an author and journalist who has published thirty-five books, both nonfiction and fiction. His recent nonfiction book, The Lost City of the Monkey God, was named a Notable Book of the Year by the New York Times. Preston previously worked as an editor at the American Museum of Natural History and taught nonfiction writing at Princeton University.

  Tim Requarth is a freelance science journalist, as well as a lecturer in science and writing at New York University. His writing has appeared in publications such as the New York Times, The Nation, The New Republic, Slate, Foreign Policy, and Scientific American. He received his PhD in neuroscience from Columbia University. He was a 2018 UC Berkeley–11th Hour Food & Farming Journalism Fellow and is a 2019–2020 Brown Institute for Media Innovation “magic grant” recipient.

 

‹ Prev