Consumer Psychology

Home > Other > Consumer Psychology > Page 14
Consumer Psychology Page 14

by Brian M Young


  9.Although the authors don’t mention this ‘being at the top of the tree’ will be different if you had to ‘climb every mountain’ to get there or just drifted up and now have ‘your head in the clouds’. I’ll leave it up to the reader to identify the cultural resonances of these expressions in single quotes.

  10.This argument is pursued further in Zhang and Risen (2014).

  11.Cuing for an experience, for example coldness, means here that the participant was shown a picture of a cold scene and asked how cold they would feel and also to write statements descriptive of their being cold.

  12.Although the authors don’t mention it the term ‘warm glow’ (a feel good factor) is sometimes cited as a reinforcer of charitable giving thus challenging the assumption that giving is pure altruism.

  13.However there is a Journal of Articles in Support of the Null Hypothesis which is issued biannually and at the time of writing is in its 13th volume.

  14.Sometimes theories in psychology are categorised as nomothetic or idiographic. Whereas idiographic approaches are interested in gathering data on individual people, a nomothetic approach deals with generalisations about groups of people.

  15.Note that these three parts of a mental act are enough to cover most of the experimental findings.

  16.They chose the value system put forward by Schwartz which is “…a universally reliable and cross-culturally valid measure of human values that has been tested on more than 200 samples in more than 60 countries from every inhabited continent” (Torelli et al., 2012, p. 93).

  17.High ceilings prime abstract thought. See Meyers-Levy and Zhu (2007).

  18.The five criteria listed here are for the experimental paradigm called priming and therefore the language used refers to observable phenomena such as stimuli and responses. It is quite legitimate however to talk of priming mental acts such as feelings, thoughts and intentions and many papers do this.

  19.However Pratkanis (1992) claimed that the exposure lasted a third of a millisecond and a subliminal message occurred every five seconds during the film (called Picnic).

  20.These figures are taken directly from Rogers’ paper.

  21.Once your respondents are split with some saying they saw, felt, or heard something, depending on what sensory threshold you are exploring and others denying they saw, felt or heard anything you are entering a zone of uncertainty. Then the temptation is take the point at which exactly half said yes and half said no as the definition, the transition as it were.

  22.The dominant sensory modality in the research is vision and hearing, touch, smell and taste and relatively unexplored.

  23.I have used the terminology ‘individualistic’ and ‘collectivistic’ as the original description but a clearer opposition that is used by Gardner et al. (1999) is ‘independent’ and ‘interdependent’.

  24.EXPERIMENT 2 in Gardner, Gabriel, and Lee (1999).

  25.The vision created by ‘subliminal perception’ of stuff coming in below the threshold is a more vivid concrete image of what might be happening.

  26.This ensures that the information is just exposed for the 13 ms. x milliseconds is written as x ms.

  27.That is, it will come to mind more often.

  28.According to Bargh (2006), one of the remarkable effects of priming is just how many psychological systems can be affected by a single prime which can spread its influence over “…perception, motivation, behavior, and evaluation” (op. cit., p. 147).

  29.Construal is taken here to mean giving sense or meaning to the self and would be a process occurring throughout life.

  30.I don’t wish to neglect the mental sharing that goes on between intimate pairs of people but these two styles of construing (independent and interdependent) predominate in the literature.

  31.The irony is that, with China ’s one-child policy (only recently being relaxed) there is a generation of individualistic, consumer oriented children growing up in a culture where collectivism is culturally dominant. See Wang (2009).

  32.Although the title of the paper hyphenates ‘self’ with ‘identity’ I’ve used the single word ‘self’ as this generic idea covers answers to the implied question ‘who am I?’

  33.It’s important to note that manipulation checks need to be carried out by for example using other tests to see if the priming did indeed work i.e. both assessments should give the same result. This experiment used such checks.

  34.How the mind grows and develops in an individual.

  35.My apologies to Ap Dijksterhuis and his colleagues who used a similar start in a very readable account of priming in Dijksterhuis, Smith, van Baaren, and Wigboldus (2005). However their description was of a rather routine experience of the local supermarket as a way into the idea that we process a lot of information unconsciously.

  36.Some readers might want to ask ‘is this the brain or the mind you’re talking about?’ The answer is unequivocal—it’s a model of the mind. There are other models of how ideas spread through the mind that are reviewed in Minton, Cornwell, and Kahle (2017).

  References

  Aaker, J., & Lee, A. Y. (2001). “I” seek pleasures and “we” avoid pains: The role of self-regulatory goals in information processing and persuasion. Journal of Consumer Research, 28, 33–49.Crossref

  Aaker, J. L. (1997). Dimensions of brand personality. Journal of Marketing Research, 34, 347–356.Crossref

  Aggarwal, P., & McGill, A. L. (2011). Partners and servants: Adopting traits of anthromorphized brands. In D. W. Dahl, G. V. Johar, & S. M. J. van Osselaer (Eds.), Advances in consumer research (Vol. 38). Duluth, MN: Association for Consumer Research. Retrieved February 9, 2018, from http://​www.​acrwebsite.​org/​volumes/​15827/​volumes/​v38/​NA-38.

  Alexander, T. M. (1989). Review: The body in the mind: The bodily basis of meaning, imagination and reason by Mark Johnson. The Journal of Speculative Philosophy (New Series), 3(2), 130–138.

  Babbes, G. S., & Malter, A. J. (1997). Special session summary embodied cognition: Towards a more realistic and productive model of mental representation. In M. Brucks & D. J. MacInnis (Eds.), Advances in consumer research (Vol. 24, pp. 39–41). Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research. Retrieved February 9, 2018, from http://​www.​acrwebsite.​org/​volumes/​8005/​volumes/​v24/​NA-24.

  Bargh, J. A. (2006). What have we been priming all these years? On the development, mechanisms, and ecology of nonconscious social behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 147–168.Crossref

  Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 230–244.Crossref

  Baxter, S., Ilicic, J., & Kulczynski, A. (2017). You see Froot, you think fruit: Examining the effectiveness of pseudohomophone priming. European Journal of Marketing, 51(5/6), 885–902.Crossref

  BBC Worldwide. (2010, June 4). Basil thrashes his car—Fawlty Towers BBC. Retrieved February 21, 2018, from https://​www.​youtube.​com/​watch?​v=​mv0onXhyLlE.

  Berger, J., & Fitzsimons, G. (2008). Dogs on the street, pumas on your feet: How cues in the environment influence product evaluation and choice. Journal of Marketing Research, 45(1), 1–14.Crossref

  Borghi, A. M., Binkofski, F., Castelfranchi, C., Cimatti, F., Scorolli, C., & Tummolini, L. (2017). The challenge of abstract concepts. Psychological Bulletin, 143(3), 263–292.Crossref

  Brasel, S. A., & Gips, J. (2011). Red Bull “gives you wings” for better or worse: A double-edged impact of brand exposure on consumer performance. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21, 57–64.Crossref

  Brennan, I. (2015). Brand induced creativity: The moderating role of cognitive ability on priming creativity through brand exposure. Academy of Marketing Studies Journal, 19(1), 120–128.

  Broadbent, D. (1958). Perception and communication. London: Pergamon Press.Crossref

  Chang, C. (2010). Message framing and interpersonal orientation at cultural
and individual levels. International Journal of Advertising, 29(5), 765–794.Crossref

  Cross, S. E., Bacon, P. L., & Morris, M. L. (2000). The relational-interdependent self-construal and relationships. Journal of Personal and Social Psychology, 78(4), 791–808.Crossref

  Davis, D. F., & Herr, P. M. (2014). From bye to buy: Homophones as a phonological route to priming. Journal of Consumer Research, 40(6), 1063–1077.Crossref

  Dijksterhuis, A., Smith, P. K., van Baaren, R. B., & Wigboldus, D. H. J. (2005). The unconscious consumer: Effects of environment on consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 15(3), 193–202.Crossref

  Doyen, S., Klein, O., Pichon, C.-L., & Cleeremans, A. (2012). Behavioral priming: It’s all in the mind, but whose mind? PLoS One, 7(1), e29081. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1371/​journal.​pone.​0029081.CrossrefPubMedPubMedCentral

  Epley, N., Waytz, A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2007). On seeing human: A three-factor theory of anthropomorphism. Psychological Review, 11(4), 864–886.Crossref

  Eskine, K. J., Kacinik, N. A., & Prinz, J. J. (2011). A bad taste in the mouth: Gustatory disgust influences moral judgment. Psychological Science, 22(3), 295–299.Crossref

  Farias, A. R., Garrido, M. V., & Semin, G. R. (2013). Converging modalities ground abstract categories: The case of politics. PLoS One, 8, e60971. Retrieved May 17, 2017, from http://​dx.​doi.​org/​10.​1371/​journal.​pone.​0060971.

  Fernyhough, C. (2006). Metaphors of mind. The Psychologist, 19(6), 356–358.

  Fitzsimons, G. M., Chartrand, T. L., & Fitzsimons, G. J. (2008). Automatic effects of brand exposure on motivated behavior: How Apple makes you “think different”. Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 21–35.Crossref

  Fournier, S. (1998). Consumers and their brands: Developing relationship theory in consumer research. Journal of Consumer Research, 24, 343–373.Crossref

  Gao, Z., & Li, N. (2013). The priming effect of news context on Chinese attitudes toward foreign brands. Journal of Asia-Pacific Business, 14(4), 268–286.Crossref

  Gardner, W. L., Gabriel, S., & Lee, A. Y. (1999). “I” value freedom, but “we” value relationships: Self-construal priming mirrors cultural differences in judgment. Psychological Science, 10(4), 321–326.Crossref

  Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: Wiley.

  Guthrie, S. (1993). Faces in the clouds: A new theory of religion. New York: Oxford University Press.

  Hong, J., & Sun, Y. (2012). Warm it up with love: The effect of physical coldness on liking of romance movies. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(2), 293–306.Crossref

  Huang, J. Y., & Bargh, J. A. (2014). The selfish goal: Autonomously operating motivational structures as the proximate cause of human judgment and behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37, 121–175.Crossref

  Hung, I. W., & Labroo, A. A. (2011). From firm muscles to firm willpower: Understanding the role of embodied cognition in self-regulation. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(6), 1046–1064.Crossref

  Janiszewski, C., & Wyer, R. S., Jr. (2014). Content and process priming: A review. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 24(1), 96–118.Crossref

  Johnson, M. (1987). The body in the mind: The bodily basis of meaning, imagination, and reason. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

  Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. London: Penguin.

  Kettle, K. L., & Häubl, G. (2011). The signature effect: Signing influences consumption-related behavior by priming self-identity. Journal of Consumer Research, 38(3), 474–489.Crossref

  Lakoff, G. (1990). Women, fire and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. London: University of Chicago Press.

  Laran, J., Dalton, A. N., & Andrade, E. B. (2011a). The curious case of behavioral backlash: Why brands produce priming effects and slogans produce reverse priming effects. Journal of Consumer Research, 37, 999–1014.Crossref

  Laran, J., Dalton, A. N., & Andrade, E. B. (2011b, November). Why consumers rebel against slogans. Harvard Business Review. Retrieved June 15, 2017, from https://​hbr.​org/​2011/​11/​why-consumers-rebel-against-slogans.

  Lee, S. H., Rotman, J. D., & Perkins, A. (2014). Embodied cognition and social consumption: Self-regulating temperature through social products and behaviors. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 24(2), 234–240.Crossref

  Meyers-Levy, J., & Zhu, R. (2007). The influence of ceiling height: The effect of priming on the type of processing that people use. Journal of Consumer Research, 34, 174–186.Crossref

  Minton, E. A., Cornwell, T. B., & Kahle, L. R. (2017). A theoretical review of consumer priming: Prospective theory, retrospective theory, and the affective-behavioral-cognitive model. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 16, 309–321.Crossref

  Morris, M., Sheldon, O., Ames, D., & Young, M. (2007). Metaphors and the market: Consequences and preconditions of agent and object metaphors in stock market commentary. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 102, 174–192.Crossref

  Mussweiler, T. (2006). Doing is for thinking! Stereotype activation by stereotypic movements. Psychological Science, 17, 17–21.Crossref

  Nelson, L. D., & Simmons, J. P. (2009). On southbound ease and northbound fees: Literal consequences of the metaphoric link between vertical position and cardinal direction. Journal of Marketing Research, 46(6), 715–724.Crossref

  Núñez, R. E., & Sweetser, E. (2006). With the future behind them: Convergent evidence from Aymara language and gesture in the cross-linguistic comparison of spatial construals of time. Cognitive Science, 30, 401–450.Crossref

  Oppezzo, M., & Schwartz, D. L. (2014). Give your ideas some legs: The positive effect of walking on creative thinking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 40(4), 1142–1152.

  Ostinelli, M., Luna, D., & Ringberg, T. (2014). When up brings you down: The effects of imagined vertical movements on motivation, performance, and consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 24(2), 271–283.Crossref

  Ouellet, M., Santiago, J., Funes, M. J., & Lupiáñez, J. (2010). Thinking about the future moves attention to the right. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(1), 17–24.PubMed

  Oyserman, D., & Lee, S. W. S. (2008). Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 311–342.Crossref

  Peters, R. S. (1962). Brett’s history of psychology (Rev. ed.). London: Allen & Unwin.

  Pratkanis, A. R. (1992). The cargo-cult science of subliminal perception. Skeptical Inquirer, 16(3), 260–272.

  Priming [Def. 9]. (n.d.). Oxford English dictionary online. Retrieved February 9, 2018, from http://​www.​oed.​com/​search?​searchType=​dictionary&​q=​Priming&​_​searchBtn=​Search.

  Rai, D., Lin, C. W., & Yang, C. M. (2017). The effects of temperature cues on charitable donation. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 34(1), 20–28.Crossref

  Risen, J. L., & Critcher, C. R. (2011). Visceral fit: While in a visceral state, associated states of the world seem more likely. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(5), 777–793.Crossref

  Rogers, S. (1993). How a publicity blitz created the myth of subliminal advertising. Public Relations Quarterly, Winter 1992–1993, 12–17.

  Rosenthal, R. (1979). The “file drawer problem” and tolerance for null results. Psychological Bulletin, 86(3), 638–641.Crossref

  Rotman, J. D., Lee, S. H., & Perkins, A. W. (2017). The warmth of our regrets: Managing regret through physiological regulation and consumption. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 27(2), 160–170.Crossref

  Shalev, I. (2014). Implicit energy loss: Embodied dryness cues influence vitality and depletion. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 24(2), 260–270.Crossref

 

‹ Prev