What Intelligence Tests Miss

Home > Other > What Intelligence Tests Miss > Page 35
What Intelligence Tests Miss Page 35

by Keith E Stanovich


  McFadden, D. (1999). Rationality for economists? Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 19, 73–105.

  McGrew, K. S. (1997). Analysis of major intelligence batteries according to a proposed comprehensive Gf-Gc framework. In D. Flanagan, J. Genshaft, & P. Harrison (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues (151–180). New York: Guilford Press.

  McGrew, K. S., & Woodcock, R. W. (2001). Technical Manual: Woodcock-Johnson III. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.

  McKenzie, C. R. M. (1994). The accuracy of intuitive judgment strategies: Covariation assessment and Bayesian inference. Cognitive Psychology, 26, 209–239.

  McKenzie, C. R. M., Liersch, M. J., & Finkelstein, S. R. (2006). Recommendations implicit in policy defaults. Psychological Science, 17, 414–420.

  McNally, R. J. (2003). Recovering memories of trauma: A view from the laboratory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 32–35.

  McNeil, B., Pauker, S., Sox, H., & Tversky, A. (1982). On the elicitation of preferences for alternative therapies. New England Journal of Medicine, 306, 1259–1262.

  Mealey, L. (1995). The sociobiology of sociopathy: An integrated evolutionary model. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18, 523–599.

  Mele, A. R. (2001). Self-deception unmasked. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

  Mele, A. R. (2003). Emotion and desire in self-deception. In A. Hatzimoysis (Ed.), Philosophy and the emotions (163–179). New York: Cambridge University Press.

  Mele, A. R., & Rawling, P. (Eds.) (2004). The Oxford handbook of rationality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  Mellers, B., Hertwig, R., & Kahneman, D. (2001). Do frequency representations eliminate conjunction effects? An exercise in adversarial collaboration. Psychological Science, 12, 269–275.

  Menand, L. (2004, August 30). The unpolitical animal. The New Yorker, 92–96.

  Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., & Laland, K. N. (2006). Towards a unified science of cultural evolution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 329–383.

  Messick, D. M. (1999). Alternative logics for decision making in social settings. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 39, 11–28.

  Messick, D. M., & Schell, T. (1992). Evidence for an equality heuristic in social decision making. Acta Psychologica, 80, 311–323.

  Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: Dynamics of will power. Psychological Review, 106, 3–19.

  Mikhail, J. (2007). Universal moral grammar: Theory, evidence, and the future. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 143–152.

  Millgram, E. (Ed.) (2001). Varieties of practical reasoning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  Mischel, W., & Ebbesen, E. B. (1970). Attention in delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16, 329–337.

  Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. L. (1989, May 26). Delay of gratification in children. Science, 244, 933–938.

  Mitchell, P., Robinson, E. J., Isaacs, J. E., & Nye, R., M. (1996). Contamination in reasoning about false belief: An instance of realist bias in adults but not children. Cognition, 59, 1–21.

  Miyake, A., Friedman, N., Emerson, M. J., & Witzki, A. H. (2000). The utility and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.

  Molden, D. C., & Higgins, E. T. (2005). Motivated thinking. In K. J. Holyoak & R. G. Morrison (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning (295–317). New York: Cambridge University Press.

  Moore, D. A. (2007). Not so above average after all: When people believe they are worse than average and its implications for theories of bias in social comparison. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 102, 42–58.

  Moore, D. A., & small, D. A. (2007). Error and bias in comparative judgment: On being both better and worse than we think we are. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 972–989.

  Moore, R. L. (1977). In search of white crows. New York: Oxford University Press.

  Moscovitch, M. (1989). Confabulation and the frontal systems: Strategic versus associative retrieval in neuropsychological theories of memory. In H. L. Roediger & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness (133– 160). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

  Moshman, D. (1994). Reasoning, metareasoning, and the promotion of rationality. In A. Demetriou & A. Efklides (Eds.), Intelligence, mind, and reasoning: Structure and development (135–150). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

  Moshman, D. (2004). From inference to reasoning: The construction of rationality. Thinking and Reasoning, 10, 221–239.

  Mostert, M. P. (2001). Facilitated communication since 1995: A review of published studies. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(3), 287–313.

  Muller, I. (1991). Hitler’s justice: The courts of the Third Reich. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  Murphy, D., & Stich, S. (2000). Darwin in the madhouse: Evolutionary psychology and the classification of mental disorders. In P. Carruthers & A. Chamberlain (Eds.), Evolution and the human mind: Modularity, language and meta-cognition (62–92). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  Musella, D. P. (2005). Gallup Poll shows that Americans’ belief in the paranormal persists. Skeptical Inquirer, 29(5), 5.

  Mussweiler, T., & Englich, B. (2005). Subliminal anchoring: Judgmental consequences and underlying mechanisms. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 98, 133–143.

  Mussweiler, T., Englich, B., & Strack, F. (2004). Anchoring effect. In R. Pohl (Ed.), Cognitive illusions: A handbook on fallacies and biases in thinking, judgment and memory (183–200). Hove, England: Psychology Press.

  Mussweiler, T., Strack, F., & Pfeiffer, T. (2000). Overcoming the inevitable anchoring effect: Considering the opposite compensates for selective accessibility. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 9, 1142–1150.

  Myers, D. G. (1990). Social psychology (3rd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.

  Myers, D. G. (2002). Intuition: Its powers and perils. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

  Naftali, T. (2007). George H. W. Bush. New York: Times Books.

  Nathan, D., & Snedeker, M. (1995). Satan’s silence: Ritual abuse and the making of a modern American witch hunt. New York: Basic Books.

  National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (2004). Traffic safety facts, 2004. Washington, DC: Author.

  National Safety Council (1990). Accident facts (1990 ed.). Chicago: Author.

  National Safety Council (2001). Report on injuries in America, 2001. Retrieved March 29, 2002, from www.nsc.org/library/rept2000.htm.

  Neisser, U. (Ed.) (1998). The rising curve: Long-term changes in IQ and related measures. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

  Neisser, U., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T., Boykin, A. W., Brody, N., Ceci, S. J., Halpern, D., Loehlin, J., Perloff, R., Sternberg, R., & Urbina, S. (1996). Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. American Psychologist, 51, 77–101.

  Neter, E., & Ben-Shakhar, G. (1989). Predictive validity of graphological inferences: A meta-analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 737–745.

  Newell, A. (1982). The knowledge level. Artificial Intelligence, 18, 87–127.

  Newell, A. (1990). Unified theories of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  Newstead, S. E., & Evans, J. St. B. T. (Eds.) (1995). Perspectives on thinking and reasoning. Hove, England: Erlbaum.

  NHTSA (2000). Vehicle design versus aggressivity, National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, U.S. Department of Transportation (DOT HS 809 194), Retrieved February 23, 2002, from NHTSA Web site: http://www-nrd.nhtsa.dot.gov/pdf/nrd-11/DOT_HS_809194.pdf.

  Nichols, S., & Stich, S. P. (2003). Mindreading: An integrated account of pretence, self-awareness, and understanding other minds. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  Nickerson, R. S. (1988). On improving thinking through instruction. In E. Z. Rothkopf (Ed.), Review of Research in Education (Vol. 15) (3–57). Was
hington, DC: American Educational Research Association.

  Nickerson, R. S. (1998). Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises. Review of General Psychology, 2, 175–220.

  Nickerson, R. S. (2004). Cognition and chance: The psychology of probabilistic reasoning. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

  Nisbett, R. E. (1993). Rules for reasoning. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

  Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

  Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.

  Norris, S. P., & Ennis, R. H. (1989). Evaluating critical thinking. Pacific Grove, CA: Midwest Publications.

  Northcraft, G. B., & Neale, M. A. (1987). Experts, amateurs, and real estate: An anchoring-and-adjustment perspective on property pricing decisions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 39, 84–97.

  Nozick, R. (1993). The nature of rationality. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

  Oaksford, M., & Chater, N. (1994). A rational analysis of the selection task as optimal data selection. Psychological Review, 101, 608–631.

  Oaksford, M., & Chater, N. (1995). Theories of reasoning and the computational explanation of everyday inference. Thinking and Reasoning, 1, 121–152.

  Oaksford, M., & Chater, N. (2007). Bayesian rationality: The probabilistic approach to human reasoning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  Oatley, K. (1992). Best laid schemes: The psychology of emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  Oatley, K. (1999). Why fiction may be twice as true as fact: Fiction as cognitive and emotional simulation. Review of General Psychology, 3, 101–117.

  Oatley, K. (2004). Emotions: A brief history. New York: Blackwell.

  O’Donoghue, T., & Rabin, M. (2000). The economics of immediate gratification. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 13, 233–250.

  Ohman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483–522.

  Ones, D. S., Viswesvaran, C., & Dilchert, S. (2005). Cognitive ability in selection decisions. In O. Wilhelm & R. W. Engle (Eds.), Handbook of understanding and measuring intelligence (431–468). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

  Over, D. E. (2000). Ecological rationality and its heuristics. Thinking and Reasoning, 6, 182–192.

  Over, D. E. (2002). The rationality of evolutionary psychology. In J. L. Bermudez & A. Millar (Eds.), Reason and nature: Essays in the theory of rationality (187–207). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  Over, D. E. (2004). Rationality and the normative/descriptive distinction. In D. J. Koehler & N. Harvey (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of judgment and decision making (3–18). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

  Ozer, D. J., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2006). Personality and the prediction of consequential outcomes. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 401–421.

  Pacini, R., & Epstein, S. (1999). The relation of rational and experiential information processing styles to personality, basic beliefs, and the ratio-bias phenomenon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 972–987.

  Pallier, G., Wilkinson, R., Danthiir, V., Kleitman, S., Knezevic, G., Stankov, L., & Roberts, R. D. (2002). The role of individual differences in the accuracy of confidence judgments. Journal of General Psychology, 129, 257–299.

  Parfit, D. (1984). Reasons and persons. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  Parker, A. M., & Fischhoff, B. (2005). Decision-making competence: External validation through an individual differences approach. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 18, 1–27.

  Parsell, D. (2004, November 13). Assault on autism. Science News, 166, 311–312.

  Paul, A. M. (2005). The cult of personality testing. New York: Free Press.

  Paulos, J. A. (1990). Innumeracy. New York: Vintage.

  Paulos, J. A. (2003). A mathematician plays the stock market. New York: Basic Books.

  Perkins, D. N. (1985). Postprimary education has little impact on informal reasoning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 562–571.

  Perkins, D. N. (1995). Outsmarting IQ: The emerging science of learnable intelligence. New York: Free Press.

  Perkins, D. N. (2002). The engine of folly. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Why smart people can be so stupid (64–85). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

  Perkins, D. N., Farady, M., & Bushey, B. (1991). Everyday reasoning and the roots of intelligence. In J. Voss, D. Perkins, & J. Segal (Eds.), Informal reasoning and education (83–105). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

  Perkins, D. N., Jay, E., & Tishman, S. (1993). Beyond abilities: A dispositional theory of thinking. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 39, 1–21.

  Perkins, D. N., & Ritchhart, R. (2004). When is good thinking? In D. Y. Dai & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), Motivation, emotion, and cognition: Integrative perspectives on intellectual functioning and development (351–384). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

  Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  Perner, J. (1998). The meta-intentional nature of executive functions and theory of mind. In P. Carruthers & J. Boucher (Eds.), Language and thought: Interdisciplinary themes (270–283). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  Perreaux, L. (2001, May 17). Drivers all edgy: Survey. National Post (Toronto), A7.

  Petrinovich, L., O’Neill, P., & Jorgensen, M. (1993). An empirical study of moral intuitions: Toward an evolutionary ethics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 467–478.

  Petry, N. M., Bickel, W. K., & Arnett, M. (1998). Shortened time horizons and insensitivity to future consequences in heroin addicts. Addiction, 93, 729–738.

  Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York: Norton.

  Piper, A. (1998). Multiple personality disorder: Witchcraft survives in the twentieth century. Skeptical Inquirer, 22(3), 44–50.

  Plomin, R., & Spinath, F. M. (2004). Intelligence: Genetics, gene, and genomics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 112–129.

  Plous, S. (1993). The psychology of judgment and decision making. New York: McGraw-hill.

  Pohl, R. (Ed.) (2004). Cognitive illusions: A handbook on fallacies and biases in thinking, judgment and memory. Hove, England: Psychology Press.

  Poletiek, F. H. (2001). Hypothesis testing behaviour. Hove, England: Psychology Press.

  Politzer, G., & Macchi, L. (2000). Reasoning and pragmatics. Mind & Society, 1, 73–93.

  Politzer, G., & Noveck, I. A. (1991). Are conjunction rule violations the result of conversational rule violations? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 20, 83–103.

  Pollock, J. L. (1995). Cognitive carpentry. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  Popper, K. R. (1972). Objective knowledge: An evolutionary approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  Postman, N. (1988). Conscientious objections. New York: vintage Books.

  Prado, J., & Noveck, I. A. (2007). Overcoming perceptual features in logical reasoning: A parametric functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 642–657.

  Prelec, D., & Bodner, R. (2003). Self-signaling and self-control. In G. F. Loewenstein, D. Read, & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Time and decision (277– 298). New York: Russell Sage.

  Prentice, R. A. (2003). Chicago man, K-T man, and the future of behavioral law and economics. Vanderbilt Law Review, 56, 1663–1777.

  Pressley, M., & Ghatala, E. S. (1990). Self-regulated learning: Monitoring learning from text. Educational Psychologist, 25, 19–33.

  Pronin, E. (2006). Perception and misperception of bias in human judgment. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 37–43.

  Pronin, E., Lin, D. Y., & Ross, L. (2002). The bias blind spot: Perceptions of bias in self versus others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 369–381.

  Pylyshyn, Z. (1984). Computation and cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  Quinn
, J. B. (2008, January 21). Help! I’m investing my 401(k). Newsweek, 82.

  Rachlin, H. (1995). Self-control: Beyond commitment. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18, 109–159.

  Rachlin, H. (2000). The science of self-control. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  Rachlinski, J. J. (2006). Cognitive errors, individual differences, and paternalism. University of Chicago Law Review, 73(1), 207–229.

  Rae-Dupree, J. (2007, December 30). Innovative minds don’t think alike. New York Times, BU3.

  Raghubir, P., & Srivastava, J. (2002). Effect of face value on product valuation in foreign currencies. Journal of Consumer Research, 29, 335–347.

  Randi, J. (1980). Flim-Flam. Buffalo, NY: Prometheus Books.

  Read, D., Loewenstein, G., & Rabin, M. (1999). Choice bracketing. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 19, 171–197.

  Reber, A. S. (1992). An evolutionary context for the cognitive unconscious. Philosophical Psychology, 5, 33–51.

  Reber, A. S. (1993). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. New York: Oxford University Press.

  Reber, A. S., Walkenfeld, F. F., & Hernstadt, R. (1991). Implicit and explicit learning: Individual differences and IQ. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 888–896.

  Reiser, M., ludwig, A., Saxe, M., & Wagner, B. (1979). An evaluation of the use of psychics in the investigation of major crimes. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 7, 18–25.

  Reiss, S., & Reiss, M. M. (2004). Curiosity and mental retardation: Beyond IQ. Mental Retardation, 42, 77–81.

  Remnick, D. (2004, September 13). The wilderness campaign. The New Yorker, 80(26), 57–83.

  Reyna, V. F. (2004). How people make decisions that involve risk. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 60–66.

  Reyna, V. F., & Farley, F. (2006). Risk and rationality in adolescent decision making. Psychological Science in the Public Interes t, 7, 1–44.

  Reyna, V. F., & Lloyd, F. J. (2006). Physician decision making and cardiac risk: Effects of knowledge, risk perception, risk tolerance, and fuzzy processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 12, 179–195.

 

‹ Prev