Book Read Free

Why People Die By Suicide

Page 16

by Thomas Joiner


  larly, for participants who were excluded during “cyberball” and re-

  ceived the psychological pain stimulus of feeling socially excluded,

  the brain scan detected activity in the anterior cingulate cortex. The

  researchers argued that evolutionary processes had recruited the phys-

  ical pain system to warn us of something potentially as dangerous as

  physical pain—namely, social exclusion or ostracism (which is like

  death, figuratively and often literally, for highly social animals). The

  need to belong is psychologically fundamental, and may have been

  fundamental in conferring evolutionary benefits to our ancestors.

  My view is that this need to belong is so powerful that, when satis-

  fied, it can prevent suicide even when perceived burdensomeness

  and the acquired ability to enact lethal self-injury are in place. By

  the same token, when the need is thwarted, risk for suicide is in-

  creased. This perspective, incidentally, is similar to that of Durkheim,

  who proposed that suicide results, in part, from failure of social inte-

  gration.

  The Desire for Death ● 119

  Some of the Stoic philosophers were proponents of rational sui-

  cide, but even they could not overcome the need to belong. For ex-

  ample, Seneca said, “Does life please you? Live on. Does it not? Go

  from whence you came. No vast wound is necessary; a mere punc-

  ture will secure your liberty. It is a bad thing (you say) to be under

  the necessity of living; but there is no necessity in the case. Thanks

  be to the gods, nobody can be compelled to live.” But when Seneca

  became seriously ill and desired suicide, he could not carry through,

  specifically because he could not bear to think of how his father

  would react. His connection to his father prevented his suicide. (I

  believe he also underestimated the ease with which suicide is

  completed—his statement that “No vast wound is necessary; a mere

  puncture will secure your liberty” contrasts with statements in Chap-

  ter 2 about the extreme difficulty of death by suicide.) Notably, Sen-

  eca later died by suicide, but well after his father’s death.

  This same sentiment is often expressed in suicide risk assessments.

  When asked about the likelihood of suicide, many patients respond

  that though they have thought of suicide, their connection to a loved

  one makes it impossible (e.g., “I couldn’t do that to so-and-so”). This

  is of course no guarantee that someone will not attempt suicide, but,

  as noted below, this clinical anecdote has some empirical support.

  Women with numerous children may be less prone to suicide than

  women with no or few children, for example.61

  In his work The Metaphysics of Ethics, Immanuel Kant writes, “To

  dispose of one’s life for some fancied end is to degrade the humanity

  subsisting in his person, and entrusted to him that he might uphold

  and preserve it.” Kant misses the perspective of the truly suicidal in-

  dividual, whose belongingness is so thwarted that she or he does not

  feel connected to humanity, and who feels that living life, not dying, degrades humanity.

  Sylvia Plath, the poet who died by suicide at the age of thirty,

  wrote, “So daddy, I’m finally through. / The black telephone’s off at

  120 ● WHY PEOPLE DIE BY SUICIDE

  the root, / The voices just can’t worm through.” I think these lines

  convey that thwarted belongingness is more than just loneliness;

  rather, it is the sense that sustaining connections are obliterated

  (“off at the root”). These lines also conflate belongingness and death

  (voices worming), which can be seen as an instance of the pattern

  mentioned previously, in which imminently suicidal people fuse

  death and life.

  Shneidman’s Ariel recalled that on the day of her self-immolation

  “I had various friends that I did know and it just seemed like they re-

  ally just didn’t have time for me.” Just before her self-immolation,

  Ariel described going to her friends’ house to return a borrowed

  toaster; she wrote, “I remember just walking in and walking through

  the house and by this time I was sobbing again. And not one word

  was said to me by these people . . . And I just walked through the

  house, put the toaster on the kitchen table and walked right out. And

  nobody touched my arm, nobody asked what’s wrong, nobody even

  gestured, and it upset me even more that this was sort of the end.”

  In his 2003 New Yorker article on suicide at the Golden Gate

  Bridge, Tad Friend quoted psychiatrist Jerome Motto on the suicide

  that affected him most. Motto said, “I went to this guy’s apartment

  afterward with the assistant medical examiner. The guy was in his

  thirties, lived alone, pretty bare apartment. He’d written a note and

  left it on his bureau. It said, ‘I’m going to walk to the bridge. If one

  person smiles at me on the way, I will not jump.’”

  Shneidman noted several other poignant examples of failed be-

  longingness in suicide: “I haven’t the love I want so bad there is noth-

  ing left” (from a forty-five-year-old married woman who died by

  overdose); “I really thought that you and little Joe were going to

  come back into my life but you didn’t” (from a twenty-year-old mar-

  ried man who died by hanging); “I just cannot live without you. I

  might as well be dead . . . I have this empty feeling inside me that is

  The Desire for Death ● 121

  killing me . . . When you left me I died inside” (from a thirty-one-

  year-old separated man who died by hanging).62

  David Reimer, the man described in Chapter 2 who was born a

  boy, raised as a girl, and then changed back to a man in adolescence,

  felt this inability to belong. A few years before his death by suicide, he

  described some of his past experiences in relating to others, and said,

  “There’s no belonging. So you’re an outcast. It doesn’t change.”63

  When asked how he had felt as a girl watching his classmates pair off

  romantically, he said, “These people looked like they knew where

  they belonged. There was no place for me to feel comfortable with

  anybody or anything.”64

  On January 5, 2002, fifteen-year-old Charles Bishop stole a small

  single-engine plane and crashed it into the Bank of America building

  in Tampa, Florida. An article from the June 14, 2004 Tampa Bay On-

  line described the final report on the boy’s death (classified as a sui-

  cide). The last line of the article read, “Bishop’s mother said he had

  no neighborhood friends, and she had not met any of his friends

  from school.”

  The empirical literature also affirms a connection between failed

  belongingness and feeling suicidal. In the sections below, this work is

  summarized, starting with research on the general connection be-

  tween depressive symptoms (one of which is suicidality) and experi-

  ences of disconnection from others.

  Behavioral Features of Depression Indicating Low Social Connection

  Connection to others can be seen in basic behavior, like eye contact

  and harmony between one person’s and another’s f
acial expressions

  or gestures. Several studies have demonstrated that depressed people

  engage in less eye contact than do nondepressed people.65 Similar

  findings have emerged with regard to non-verbal gestures. For exam-

  ple, as compared to others, depressed people may engage in less

  122 ● WHY PEOPLE DIE BY SUICIDE

  head-nodding during conversation; head-nodding is a gesture af-

  firming connection that communication partners find rewarding.66

  Depressed and suicidal people have trouble engaging in the subtle

  back-and-forth dance of nonverbal communication—they often do

  not return eye contact, do not display animated facial expression in

  reactions to others, and do not use gestures like head-nodding that

  others find affirming and engaging.

  Research on Social Isolation, Disconnection, and Suicidal Behavior

  In the last section on perceived burdensomeness, I noted that rela-

  tively few studies had empirically assessed the connection between

  burdensomeness and suicidality, though all studies were supportive

  of the link. The situation is different with regard to failed belonging-

  ness: The fact that those who die by suicide experience isolation and

  withdrawal before their deaths is among the clearest in all the litera-

  ture on suicide.

  An intriguing example involves language use by poets who died by

  suicide compared to nonsuicidal poets as the poets’ deaths neared.67

  These researchers used the Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count

  (LIWC), mentioned earlier, to analyze text into its components—

  for example, tendency to use action verbs, words denoting negative

  emotion, and so on. Their results suggested escalating interpersonal

  disconnection in the suicidal poets but not the poets who died by

  other means. Specifically, as the suicidal poets’ deaths approached,

  their use of interpersonal pronouns (e.g., “we”) decreased noticeably.

  Similarly, Shneidman reports on a young man who had survived a

  self-inflicted gunshot wound and later wrote, “Those around me

  were as shadows, bare apparitions, but I was not actually conscious

  of them, only aware of myself and my plight. Death swallowed me

  long before I pulled the trigger. I was locked within myself.”68

  I recently conducted a study using the LIWC software to examine

  psychological variables associated with suicidal behavior by analyz-

  The Desire for Death ● 123

  ing differences in linguistic patterns in two literary characters in Wil-

  liam Faulkner’s novel The Sound and the Fury. These characters, the brothers Quentin and Jason Compson, each have a section in the

  novel that is written from a first person, stream-of-consciousness

  point of view. Quentin’s section is written the day before he dies by

  suicide. Jason’s section is written approximately ten years later. Each

  section of the novel reveals characters’ inner thoughts and cognitive

  processes as they are occurring. Accordingly, an analysis of psycho-

  logical variables over time as written by Faulkner for Quentin and Ja-

  son seemed feasible.

  I wondered whether Faulkner was skilled enough to accurately

  portray psychological aspects of approaching suicide, phenomena

  that were not well elucidated by those studying suicide at the time

  Faulkner was writing The Sound and the Fury (published in 1932).

  Indeed, we found that Quentin’s use of social words decreased as his

  death by suicide approached; Jason’s use of social words did not

  change over time.69 Faulkner accurately portrayed relatively poorly

  understood, intense, and rare psychological processes—still more in-

  dication of his literary genius.

  Work on nonfictional people reaches similar conclusions. In a sur-

  vey on several hundred community participants, as well as on five

  high-suicide-risk groups (e.g., general psychiatric patients, incarcer-

  ated psychiatric patients), social isolation stood out as a correlate of

  suicidal ideation (perceived burdensomeness toward family was also

  a strong correlate).70 Similarly, in a study of psychiatrists’ reports on

  their patients’ suicides, three variables were seen as frequently pres-

  ent in the month preceding suicide: feeling a burden on others; social

  withdrawal; and help negation.71 Help negation (the tendency to

  thwart help, especially therapeutic help) has been viewed as a process

  of interpersonal disconnection, and as such, may represent an in-

  stance of thwarted belongingness.72

  Conner and colleagues assessed men with alcohol dependence

  124 ● WHY PEOPLE DIE BY SUICIDE

  who died by suicide. Among the risk factors for completed suicide

  were living alone and loss of a partner within the last month or two

  before death.73 Similarly, a comparison of those who died by suicide

  and those who died by other means revealed that those who died by

  suicide were more likely to have been recently separated and living

  alone.74 Significant others of people who had recently attempted sui-

  cide pointed to loneliness in the patients as an important factor in

  their suicide attempt.75

  A study of African-American women examined reasons for the as-

  sociation between types of childhood maltreatment and suicidal be-

  havior. Of various factors examined, alienation (defined as inability

  to establish basic trust and achieve stable and satisfying relation-

  ships) was the most robust, fully explaining the link between all

  forms of childhood maltreatment and later suicidal behavior.76

  Marital Status, Parenting, and Suicidality

  As the studies on social isolation might suggest, nonmarried status

  is a demographic risk factor for suicide. The majority of deaths by

  suicide among Native Americans of the Apache, Navajo, and Pueblo

  tribes, for instance, were of single people.77 Statistics indicate the

  following suicide rates in the United States in 1999: divorced—32.7

  per 100,000; widowed—19.7 per 100,000; single—17.8 per 100,000;

  married—10.6 per 100,000.78 These national statistics are of course

  open to many interpretations, but they are consistent with the view

  that belongingness (as indicated by married status) is a suicide

  buffer, whereas thwarted belongingness (as indicated by nonmarried

  status) is a risk for death by suicide. This is particularly evident

  with regard to divorce (which confers a threefold increase in risk rel-

  ative to married status). In context of the model proposed here, it is

  tempting to speculate that suicide rates among divorced people are

  particularly high because divorce can affect both basic feelings of ef-

  fectiveness (e.g., feeling a failure as a spouse) and basic feelings of

  The Desire for Death ● 125

  connectedness (losing social contact not only with a spouse, but po-

  tentially with the spouse’s family, with children, and with friends

  previously shared with the spouse).

  These statistics on marital status converge with a literature in-

  spired by Durkheim’s emphasis on failures of social integration as a

  source for suicide.79 For example, a study of suicidality and family

  and parental functionin
g in over 4,000 high school students in Ice-

  land concluded that those adolescents who were well integrated into

  their families thereby derived protection from suicide; the indices re-

  lated to family integration (cf. belongingness) wielded stronger in-

  fluence on suicidality than did indices related to how the parents

  were functioning.80

  With regard to connections with children, there is evidence that

  having large numbers of children protects against suicide. In a study

  of nearly a million women in Norway, over 1,000 died by suicide

  during a fifteen-year follow-up. Women with six or more children

  had one-fifth the risk of death by suicide as compared to other

  women.81 The suicide rates in Canada’s provinces are associated with

  birth rates, such that more births correspond with fewer suicides,

  consistent with the possibility that ties to new children buffer against

  suicidality.82 In a very persuasive study on this point on over 18,000

  Danish people who died by suicide and over 370,000 matched con-

  trols, having children, especially young children, was protective

  against suicide, even when accounting for powerful suicide-related

  variables like marital status and psychiatric disorder.83 To my knowl-

  edge, no studies like this have been conducted specifically on fathers,

  parenthood, and suicide.

  The result on mothers, parenthood, and suicide may even extend

  to pregnancy. Marzuk and colleagues examined the autopsy reports

  for all women who died by suicide in New York City from 1990 to

  1993 and compared them to overall mortality statistics in age- and

  race-matched women in New York during the same time period.

  126 ● WHY PEOPLE DIE BY SUICIDE

  During the study, there were 315 women who died by suicide in New

  York City, six of whom were pregnant, which was one-third the

  number that was expected given New York City female population

  rates.84 These researchers concluded that pregnancy conferred pro-

  tection from suicide; I would suggest that the protective influence in-

  volved feelings of connection to the baby, as well as feeling needed by

  the baby and thus not a burden.

  Pregnancy, by itself, is no solution to longstanding feelings of dis-

  connection and perceived burdensomeness, however. In fact, consis-

  tent with this assertion, my colleagues and I found that initially pes-

 

‹ Prev