The Big Picture

Home > Other > The Big Picture > Page 56
The Big Picture Page 56

by Carroll, Sean M.


  27

  The connectome is simply the list of every single neuron in the brain,

  28

  along with all of the connections between them. It’s a system of impressive

  29

  complexity: the human brain contains roughly 85 billion neurons, each of

  30

  which is connected to a thousand or more other neurons, so we’re talking

  31

  about a hundred trillion or more connections in total. It’s hard to look into

  32

  a real human brain and discern all of those connections— but that’s exactly

  33

  the goal of several ongoing neuroscience research projects. Fully character-

  34

  izing the human connectome would require something like a million mil-

  S35

  lion gigabytes of information.

  N36

  329

  Big Picture - UK final proofs.indd 329

  20/07/2016 10:02:52

  T H E B IG PIC T U R E

  01

  Every neuron gleans input from other neurons, and occasionally from

  02

  the outside world. Given that input, it decides whether to fire. Firing is a yes

  03

  or no question— it either happens or it doesn’ t— but the input the neuron

  04

  receives can be quite rich. Very roughly, a neuron will “listen” to its input

  05

  for about 40 milliseconds at a time, and each incoming signal takes one

  06

  millisecond to transfer. That’s a huge amount of information. Forty sepa-

  07

  rate inputs, from a couple of thousand different synapses, resulting in

  08

  roughly 40 x 2,000 = 80,000 “bits” of information, or about 280,000 possible

  09

  messages a neuron could receive before it decides whether to fire or not. It’s

  10

  not simply “If I get more than the appropriate number of input signals, I

  11

  will fire”; some signals increase the chance of firing, some decrease it, and

  12

  the signals interact in complicated ways.

  13

  Knowing the complete human connectome wouldn’t, by itself, come

  14

  close to telling us everything we want to know about how human brains

  15

  think. Not all neurons are the same, so knowing how they are connected

  16

  isn’t everything there is to know. Scientists have completely mapped the

  17

  connectome of one multicellular organism: the tiny C. elegans nematode, a

  18

  flatworm whose most common form has precisely 959 cells, 302 of which

  19

  are neurons. We know how all of those neurons fit together— about 7,000

  20

  connections in total— but that doesn’t tell us what the flatworm is think-

  21

  ing. It’s like we know the highway map, but not the traffic patterns. Maybe

  22

  someday we’ll be able to read the nematode’s mind.

  23

  24

  25

  26

  27

  28

  29

  30

  31

  32

  33

  34

  35S

  The connectome of the C. elegans nematode, as represented in a computer model

  36N

  from the OpenWorm project. (Courtesy of Chris Grove, Caltech)

  330

  Big Picture - UK final proofs.indd 330

  20/07/2016 10:02:53

  t h E b A b b l I n g b R A I n

  People change over time, and our connectomes change along with us.

  01

  The strength of the connections evolves, as the repeated firing of certain

  02

  signals increases the chances that specific synapses will fire again in the

  03

  future. We believe that memories are formed in this way, by synapses grow-

  04

  ing and shrinking in strength in response to stimuli. Neuropsychiatrist

  05

  Eric Kandel shared the 2000 Nobel Prize in Medicine for his detailed in-

  06

  vestigation of how this happens in a particular organism, the humble sea

  07

  slug. Slugs aren’t great at remembering things, but Kandel trained them to

  08

  recognize certain simple stimuli. He then showed that these new memories

  09

  were connected to a change in the synthesis of proteins in the neurons,

  10

  which led to alterations in their shape. Short- term memories were associ-

  11

  ated with synapses being strengthened, while long- term memories came

  12

  from entirely new synapses being created.

  13

  More recently, neuroscientists have been able to directly observe neu-

  14

  rons in mice growing and connecting as they learned how to perform new

  15

  tasks. Impressively (or disturbingly, depending on your perspective), they

  16

  have also been able to remove memories from mice by weakening specific

  17

  synapses, and even artificially implanting false memories by directly stimu-

  18

  lating individual nerve cells with electrodes. Memories are physical things,

  19

  located in your brain.

  20

  A connectome is like a map of the countries of the world. It’s not nearly

  21

  enough to allow us to understand politics, but knowing the information

  22

  contained therein is an important part of the bigger task. Having a good

  23

  map won’t stop you from getting lost, but it might help you find your

  24

  way home.

  25

  26

  •

  27

  One of the most crucial features of the brain is that it’s not simply an undif-

  28

  ferentiated mess of connected neurons. The connectome is a network, but

  29

  it’s a hierarchical network— groups of neurons are connected together, and

  30

  those groups are then connected, and so on up to the entire brain. The

  31

  babble of consciousness, with different mental modules offering input and

  32

  being stitched together to make our aware self, is reflected in the workings

  33

  of the brain. Different parts have their own jobs to do, but it’s only when

  34

  they come together that we find a conscious person.

  S35

  There are various pieces of evidence for this, some of which come from

  N36

  331

  Big Picture - UK final proofs.indd 331

  20/07/2016 10:02:53

  T H E B IG PIC T U R E

  01

  studies of what happens when we lose consciousness: when we sleep, or

  02

  when we’re under anesthesia. One study, for example, gave a small magnetic

  03

  stimulation to local regions of patients’ brains. Effects of the signal were

  04

  then measured as they propagated through the brain. When the patients

  05

  were conscious, the signal induced responses all over the brain; in uncon-

  06

  scious subjects the responses were confined to a limited r
egion near the

  07

  initial stimulus. Results like this are of much more than academic interest:

  08

  doctors have long sought a way of telling whether a patient under anesthesia

  09

  or suffering from brain damage was truly unconscious, or merely unable to

  10

  move and communicate with the outside world.

  11

  To say that the connectome is a hierarchical network is to say that it lies

  12

  somewhere between being maximally connected (every neuron is talking

  13

  to every other neuron) and minimally connected (every neuron talks only

  14

  to its immediate neighbors). As far as we can tell, the connectome is what

  15

  mathematicians call a small- world network. The name comes from the fa-

  16

  mous six- degrees-of-separation experiment by psychologist Stanley Mil-

  17

  gram. He found that randomly chosen people in Omaha, Nebraska, were

  18

  linked to a specific person living in Boston, Massachusetts, by an average of

  19

  about six first- name relationships. In network theory, we say that a network

  20

  has the small- world property when most nodes are not directly connected

  21

  to one another, but each one can be reached from any other one by a small

  22

  number of steps.

  23

  That’s what we find in the connectome. Neurons tend to be connected

  24

  to nearby neurons, but there are also connections relatively far away. Small-

  25

  world networks show up in many contexts, including connections between

  26

  websites, electrical power grids, and networks of personal friendships.

  27

  That’s not an accident: this kind of organization seems to represent an op-

  28

  timum of efficiency for certain tasks, allowing processing to be done locally

  29

  and results to spread quickly throughout the system. It is also robust to

  30

  damage; knocking out a few connections doesn’t appreciably alter the sys-

  31

  tem’s capacity. It’s a perfect fit for the squabbling modules inside our brains.

  32

  One way of thinking about a small- world network is to say that it has

  33

  “structure at all scales.” It is not simply a bunch of neurons grouped into a

  34

  ball, with those balls connected to one another. Rather, it’s neurons con-

  35S

  nected into groups, connected into bigger groups, into even bigger groups,

  36N

  and so on. There is some indication that this kind of arrangement describes

  332

  Big Picture - UK final proofs.indd 332

  20/07/2016 10:02:54

  t h E b A b b l I n g b R A I n

  not only the spatial organization of the connectome but also how signals in

  01

  the brain come and go in time. Small signals happen relatively frequently,

  02

  medium- sized ones less often, and very big ones relatively rarely.

  03

  Physicists say that systems with this kind of hierarchical behavior are at

  04

  a critical point. It’s a ubiquitous phenomenon in the study of phase transi-

  05

  tions, since systems become critical right as they are about to change from

  06

  one phase to another. When water boils, there are many small bubbles,

  07

  fewer larger ones, and so on. Criticality can be thought of as a sweet spot

  08

  between boring order and useless chaos. As neurophysiologist Dante

  09

  Chialvo put it, “A brain that is not critical is a brain that does exactly the

  10

  same thing every minute, or, in the other extreme, is so chaotic that it does

  11

  a completely random thing, no matter what the circumstances. That is the

  12

  brain of an idiot.”

  13

  In both space and time, then, the evidence we have to date indicates that

  14

  our brains are complex systems organized in such a way as to take maxi-

  15

  mum advantage of their complexity. Given how impressive our brains are

  16

  at carrying out complicated tasks, that should come as no surprise.

  17

  18

  •

  19

  We could study the brain in exquisite detail, characterizing every neuron

  20

  and mapping every connection, and still not convince ourselves that the

  21

  brain accounts for the mind, the actual thinking of a human being. Back in

  22

  chapter 26 we talked about Princess Elisabeth’s objections to Descartes’s

  23

  picture of an immaterial soul interacting with the physical body, perhaps

  24

  through the pineal gland. As interesting as those objections were, they don’t

  25

  necessarily close the deal until we can directly connect what happens in the

  26

  brain to what we think of as our identities as persons. Over the years psy-

  27

  chology and neuroscience have made great strides in doing just that.

  28

  We’ve already seen that memories are physically encoded in the brain.

  29

  It’s unsurprising, then, that our sensory perceptions are likewise encoded

  30

  there. This is obviously true in some crude way, as the magnetic fields stick-

  31

  ing out of my head demonstrated. But scientists have made advances re-

  32

  cently in extracting quite detailed images of what patients are seeing, just

  33

  by looking at what their brains are doing. By using fMRI images to deter-

  34

  mine what parts of the brain are firing when subjects are looking at images,

  S35

  or watching videos, neuroscientists can construct a template from which

  N36

  333

  Big Picture - UK final proofs.indd 333

  20/07/2016 10:02:54

  T H E B IG PIC T U R E

  01

  they can reconstruct images directly from the fMRI data, without “cheat-

  02

  ing” by knowing what the subjects are watching. It’s not mind reading, at

  03

  least not yet; we can make crude representations of what people are looking

  04

  at, but not what they are imagining inside their heads. Perhaps that’s just a

  05

  matter of time.

  06

  None of this will necessarily convince a determined Cartesian dualist

  07

  who wants to believe in immaterial souls. Of course, they will admit, some-

  08

  thing happens in the brain as we think and perceive the world. But that’s not

  09

  all that happens. The experiencing, the feeling, the actual soul of a person—

  10

  that’s something else entirely. Perhaps the brain is like a radio receiver. Al-

  11

  tering it or damaging it will change how it plays, but that doesn’t mean that

  12

  the original signal is being created
inside the radio itself.

  13

  That idea doesn’t really hold up either. Damaging a radio might hurt our

  14

  reception, making it hard to pick up our favorite station. But it doesn’t turn

  15

  that station from heavy- metal music into a smooth- jazz format. Damag-

  16

  ing the brain, on the other hand, can change who a person is at a fundamen-

  17

  tal level.

  18

  Consider what’s known as the Capgras delusion. Patients suffering from

  19

  this syndrome have damage to the part of the brain that connects two other

  20

  parts: the temporal cortex, associated with recognizing other people, and

  21

  the limbic system, which is in charge of feelings and emotions. A person

  22

  who develops Capgras delusion will be able to recognize people they know,

  23

  but will no longer feel whatever emotional connection they used to have

  24

  with them. (It is the flip side of prosopagnosia, which involves a loss of the

  25

  ability to recognize people.)

  26

  You can imagine what this would do to a person. One patient, “Mrs. D,”

  27

  began to suffer from Capgras delusion at the age of seventy- four. Whenever

  28

  she would see her husband, she would recognize this person, including all

  29

  of the mental associations that said “this is my husband”— but she no lon-

  30

  ger felt any affection or love toward him, merely indifference. But she knew

  31

  that she should have feelings for him, so her brain came up with a clever

  32

  reconciliation of the inconsistency: this man wasn’t really her husband, he

  33

  was an impostor who looked just like him.

  34

  Mrs. D was not a unique case. There are many other examples of people

  35S

  suffering from some sort of brain damage, and having their emotional states

  36N

  or personalities dramatically altered thereby. That doesn’t prove beyond any

  334

  Big Picture - UK final proofs.indd 334

  20/07/2016 10:02:54

  t h E b A b b l I n g b R A I n

  possible doubt that the mind is nothing more than a way of talking about

  01

  what happens in the physical brain. But it should work to lower our cre-

  02

  dence in old- fashioned Cartesian dualism to a very small value indeed.

  03

  That leaves us either with physicalism— the world, including people, is

  04

  purely physical— or some newfangled form of non- Cartesian dualism. To

  05

  clean up that final question, we need to think more about what it means to

  06

  be a conscious, experiencing person.

 

‹ Prev