The Gap

Home > Other > The Gap > Page 42
The Gap Page 42

by Thomas Suddendorf


  Sagan, C. (1980). Cosmos. New York: Random House.

  Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 9, 185–211.

  Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1986). Ape language. New York: Columbia University Press.

  Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., et al. (1980). Do apes use language? American Scientist, 68, 49–61.

  Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., et al. (1993). Language comprehension in ape and child. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58, 1–222.

  Scally, A., et al. (2012). Insights into hominid evolution from the gorilla genome sequence. Nature, 483, 169–175.

  Scarf, D., et al. (2012). Social evaluation or simple association? PLOS One, 7, e42698.

  Schacter, D. L. (1999). The seven sins of memory: Insights from psychology and cognitive neuroscience. American Psychologist, 54, 182–203.

  Schacter, D. L., et al. (2007). Remembering the past to imagine the future: The prospective brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 657–661.

  Schmandt-Besserat, D. (1992). Before writing. Austin: University of Texas Press.

  Schusterman, R. J., & Gisiner, R. (1988). Artificial language comprehension in dolphins and sea lions: Essential cognitive skills. Psychological Record, 38, 311–348.

  Schwarz, E. (1929). The occurrence of the chimpanzee south of the Congo River. Revue de Zoologie et de Botanique Africaines, 16, 425–426.

  Sear, R., & Mace, R. (2008). Who keeps children alive? A review of the effects of kin on child survival. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 1–18.

  Seed, A. M., et al. (2009). Chimpanzees solve the trap problem when the confound of tool-use is removed. Journal of Experimental Psychology—Animal Behavior Processes, 35, 23–34.

  Semaw, S. (2000). The world’s oldest stone artefacts from Gona, Ethiopia: Their implications for understanding stone technology and patterns of human evolution between 2.6–1.5 million years ago. Journal of Archaeological Science, 27, 1197–1214.

  Senut, B., et al. (2001). First hominid from the Miocene (Lukeino Formation, Kenya). Comptes Rendus de l’Academie des Sciences Serie Ii Fascicule A-Sciences de la Terre et des Planetes, 332, 137–144.

  Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2012). The evolutionary origins of friendship. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 153–177.

  Shahaeian, A., et al. (2011). Culture and the sequence of steps in theory of mind development. Developmental Psychology, 47, 1239–1247.

  Shermer, M. (1997). Why people believe weird things. New York: W. H. Freeman.

  Shettleworth, S. J. (2010). Clever animals and killjoy explanations in comparative psychology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 477–481.

  Shipton, C. (2010). Imitation and shared intentionality in the Acheulean. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 20, 197–210.

  Shweder, R. A., et al. (1987). Cultural and moral development. In J. Kagan & S. Lamb (Eds.), The emergence of morality in young children (pp. 1–83). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

  Silberberg, A., & Kearns, D. (2009). Memory for the order of briefly presented numerals in humans as a function of practice. Animal Cognition, 12, 405–407.

  Silk, J. B. (2010). Fellow feeling. American Scientist, 98, 158–160.

  Silk, J. B., et al. (2005). Chimpanzees are indifferent to the welfare of unrelated group members. Nature, 437, 1357–1359.

  Singer, P. (2002). One world: The ethics of globalization. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

  Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

  Sleator, R. D. (2010). The human superorganism: Of microbes and men. Medical Hypotheses, 74, 214–215.

  Slobodchikoff, C. N., et al. (2009). Prairie dog alarm calls encode labels about predator colors. Animal Cognition, 12, 435–439.

  Smil, V. (2002). The earth’s biosphere. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  Smith, J. D., et al. (1995). The uncertain response in the bottlenosed dolphin (Tusiops truncatus). Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 124, 391–408.

  Smith, J. D., et al. (2012). The highs and lows of theoretical interpretation in animal-metacognition research. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 367, 1297–1309.

  Smith, J. N., et al. (2008). Songs of male humpback whales, Megaptera novaeangliae, are involved in intersexual interactions. Animal Behaviour, 76, 467–477.

  Smith, T. M., et al. (2007). Earliest evidence of modern human life history in North African early Homo sapiens. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 6128–6133.

  Soares, P., et al. (2009). Correcting for purifying selection: An improved human mitochondrial molecular clock. American Journal of Human Genetics, 84, 740–759.

  Stamp Dawkins, M. (2012). What do animals want? The Edge. Retrieved from http://edge.org/conversation/what-do-animals-want

  Stanford, C., et al. (2013). Biological Anthropology (3rd ed.). Boston: Pearson.

  Staudinger, U. M., & Gluck, J. (2011). Psychological wisdom research: Commonalities and differences in a growing field. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 215–241.

  Stedman, H. H., et al. (2004). Myosin gene mutation correlates with anatomical changes in the human lineage. Nature, 428, 415–418.

  Sterelny, K. (2003). Thought in a hostile world: The evolution of human cognition. Malden, MA: Blackwell.

  Sterelny, K. (2010). Moral nativism: A sceptical response. Mind and Language, 25, 279–297.

  Sternberg, R. J. (1999). Successful intelligence: Finding a balance. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 436–442.

  Stevens, J. R., & Hauser, M. D. (2004). Why be nice? Psychological constraints on the evolution of cooperation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 60–65.

  Stewart, J. R., & Stringer, C. B. (2012). Human evolution out of Africa: The role of refugia and climate change. Science, 335, 1317–1321.

  Stone, R. (2011). Last-ditch effort to save embattled ape. Science, 331, 390.

  Stout, D. (2011). Stone toolmaking and the evolution of human culture and cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 366, 1050–1059.

  Stringer, C. B., et al. (2008). Neanderthal exploitation of marine mammals in Gibraltar. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105, 14319–14324.

  Strum, S. C. (2008). Perspectives on de Waal’s Primates and philosophers: How morality evolved. Current Anthropology, 49, 701–702.

  Suddendorf, T. (1999a). Children’s understanding of the relation between delayed video representation and current reality: A test for self-awareness? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72, 157–176.

  Suddendorf, T. (1999b). The rise of the metamind. In M. C. Corballis & S. E. G. Lea (Eds.), The descent of mind: Psychological perspectives on hominid evolution (pp. 218–260). London: Oxford University Press.

  Suddendorf, T. (2003). Early representational insight: Twenty-four-month-olds can use a photo to find an object in the world. Child Development, 74, 896–904.

  Suddendorf, T. (2004). How primatology can inform us about the evolution of the human mind. Australian Psychologist, 39, 180–187.

  Suddendorf, T. (2006). Foresight and evolution of the human mind. Science, 312, 1006–1007.

  Suddendorf, T. (2008). Explaining human cognitive autapomorphies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 147–148.

  Suddendorf, T. (2010a). Episodic memory versus episodic foresight: Similarities and differences. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews Cognitive Science, 1, 99–107.

  Suddendorf, T. (2010b). Linking yesterday and tomorrow: Preschoolers’ ability to report temporally displaced events. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 491–498.

  Suddendorf, T. (2011). Evolution, lies and foresight biases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34, 38–39.

  Suddendorf, T., & Busby, J. (2003). Mental time travel in animals? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 391–396.

  Suddendorf, T., & Busby, J. (2005). Making decisi
ons with the future in mind: Developmental and comparative identification of mental time travel. Learning and Motivation, 36, 110–125.

  Suddendorf, T., & Butler, D. L. (2013). The nature of visual self-recognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 121–127.

  Suddendorf, T., & Collier-Baker, E. (2009). The evolution of primate visual self-recognition: evidence of absence in lesser apes. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 276, 1671–1677.

  Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (1997). Mental time travel and the evolution of the human mind. Genetic Social and General Psychology Monographs, 123, 133–167.

  Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 299–313+335–351.

  Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2008a). Episodic memory and mental time travel. In E. Dere et al. (Eds.), Handbook of Episodic Memory (Vol. 18, pp. 31–42) Amsterdam: Elsevier.

  Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2008b). New evidence for animal foresight? Animal Behaviour, 75, e1–e3.

  Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2010). Behavioural evidence for mental time travel in nonhuman animals. Behavioural Brain Research, 215, 292–298.

  Suddendorf, T., & Dong, A. (2013). On the evolution of imagination and design. In M. Taylor (Ed.), Oxford handbook of the development of imagination (pp. 453–467). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  Suddendorf, T., & Fletcher-Flinn, C. M. (1999). Children’s divergent thinking improves when they understand false beliefs. Creativity Research Journal, 12, 115–128.

  Suddendorf, T., & Moore, C. (2011). Introduction to the special issue: The development of episodic foresight. Cognitive Development, 26, 295–298.

  Suddendorf, T., & Whiten, A. (2001). Mental evolution and development: Evidence for secondary representation in children, great apes and other animals. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 629–650.

  Suddendorf, T., & Whiten, A. (2003). Reinterpreting the mentality of apes. In K. Sterelny & J. Fitness (Eds.), From mating to mentality: Evaluating evolutionary psychology (pp. 173–196). New York: Psychology Press.

  Suddendorf, T., et al. (2007). Visual self-recognition in mirrors and live videos: Evidence for a developmental asynchrony. Cognitive Development, 22, 185–196.

  Suddendorf, T., et al. (2009a). How great is great ape foresight? Animal Cognition, 12, 751–754.

  Suddendorf, T., et al. (2009b). Mental time travel and the shaping of the human mind. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 364, 1317–1324.

  Suddendorf, T., et al. (2011). Children’s capacity to remember a novel problem and to secure its future solution. Developmental Science, 14, 26–33.

  Suddendorf, T., et al. (2012). If I could talk to the animals. Metascience, 21, 253–267.

  Suddendorf, T., et al. (2013). Is newborn imitation developmentally homologous to later social-cognitive skills? Developmental Psychobiology, 55, 52–58.

  Surbeck, M., & Hohmann, G. (2008). Primate hunting by bonobos at LuiKotale, Salonga National Park. Current Biology, 18, R906–R907.

  Suwa, G., et al. (2009). The Ardipithecus ramidus skull and its implications for hominid origins. Science, 326, 68e1–68e7.

  Svetlova, M., et al. (2010). Toddlers’ prosocial behavior: From instrumental to empathic to altruistic helping. Child Development, 81, 1814–1827.

  Swartz, K. B., et al. (1999). Comparative aspects of mirror self-recognition in great apes. In S. T. Parker, R.W. Mitchell, & M.L. Boccia (Eds.), The mentalities of gorillas and orangutans (pp. 283–294). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  Swisher, C. C., et al. (1996). Latest Homo erectus of Java: Potential contemporaneity with Homo sapiens in southeast Asia. Science, 274, 1870–1874.

  Szagun, G. (1978). On the frequency of use of tenses in English and German children’s spontaneous speech. Child Development, 49, 898–901.

  Tardif, S. D. (1997). The bioenergetics of parental behavior and the evolution of alloparental care in marmosets and tamarins: In N. G. Solomon & J. A. French (Eds.), Cooperative breeding in mammals (pp. 11–33). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  Taylor, A. H., et al. (2007). Spontaneous metatool use by New Caledonian crows. Current Biology, 17, 1504–1507.

  Taylor, A. H., et al. (2009). Do New Caledonian crows solve physical problems through causal reasoning? Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 276, 247–254.

  Taylor, A. H., et al. (2010). An investigation into the cognition behind spontaneous string pulling in New Caledonian crows. PLOS One, 5, e9345.

  Taylor, A. H., et al. (2011). New Caledonian Crows learn the functional properties of novel tool types. PLOS One, 6, e26887.

  Taylor, A. H., et al. (2012). New Caledonian crows reason about hidden causal agents. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109, 16389–16391.

  Taylor, M., et al. (1994). Children’s understanding of knowledge acquisition: The tendency for children to report that they have always known what they have just learned. Child Development, 65, 1581–1604.

  Taylor, T. (2010). The artificial ape: How technology changed the course of human evolution. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

  Tedeschi, J. T. (1981). Impression management theory and social psychological research. New York: Academic Press.

  Tennie, C., et al. (2004). Imitation versus emulation in great apes. Folia Primatologica, 75, 728.

  Terrace, H. S. (1979). Nim. New York: Knopf.

  Thieme, H. (1997). Lower Palaeolithic hunting spears from Germany. Nature, 385, 807–810.

  Thompson-Cannino, J., et al. (2009). Picking cotton: Our memoir of injustice and redemption. New York: St. Martin’s Press.

  Thomson, R., et al. (2000). Recent common ancestry of human Y chromosomes: Evidence from DNA sequence data. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97, 7360–7365.

  Thorpe, I. J. N. (2003). Anthropology, archaeology, and the origin of warfare. World Archaeology, 35, 145–165.

  Thorpe, S. K. S., et al. (2007). Origin of human bipedalism as an adaptation for locomotion on flexible branches. Science, 316, 1328–1331.

  Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 20, 410–433.

  Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review, 55, 189–208.

  Tomasello, M. (1999). The human adaptation for culture. Annual Review of Anthropology, 28, 509–529.

  Tomasello, M. (2009). Why we cooperate. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (1997). Primate cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.

  Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2005). The emergence of social cognition in three young chimpanzees. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 70, 1–132.

  Tomasello, M., et al. (1993a). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495–552.

  Tomasello, M., et al. (1993b). Imitative learning of actions on objects by children, chimpanzees, and enculturated chimpanzees. Child Development, 64, 1688–1705.

  Tomasello, M., et al. (1999). Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, follow gaze direction geometrically. Animal Behaviour, 58, 769–777.

  Tomasello, M., et al. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–691.

  Tooby, J., & DeVore, I. (1987). The reconstruction of hominid behavioral evolution through strategic modelling. In W. Kinzey (Ed.), The evolution of human behavior: Primate models (pp. 183–238). Albany: State University of New York Press.

  Toups, M. A., et al. (2011). Origin of clothing lice indicates early clothing use by anatomically modern humans in Africa. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 28, 29–32.

  Trinkaus, E. (1995). Neanderthal mortality patterns. Journal of Archaeological Science, 22, 121–142.

  Trivers, R. L. (1971). Evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Rev
iew of Biology, 46, 35–57.

  Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology, 26, 1–12.

  Tulving, E. (2005). Episodic memory and autonoesis: Uniquely human? In H. S. Terrace & J. Metcalfe (Eds.), The missing link in cognition (pp. 3–56). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  Turney, C. S. M., et al. (2008). Late-surviving megafauna in Tasmania, Australia, implicate human involvement in their extinction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105, 12150–12153.

  Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131.

  Twain, M. (1906). What is man? Retrieved from: http://www.gutenberg.org/ebooks/70

  Ueno, A., & Matsuzawa, T. (2004). Food transfer between chimpanzee mothers and their infants. Primates, 45, 231–239.

  Ujhelyi, M., et al. (2000). Observations on the behavior of gibbons (Hylobates leucogenys, H. gabriellae, and H. lar) in the presence of mirrors. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 114, 253–262.

  Ungar, P. S., & Sponheimer, M. (2011). The diets of early hominins. Science, 334, 190–193.

  Utami, S. S., et al. (2002). Male bimaturism and reproductive success in Sumatran orang-utans. Behavioral Ecology, 13, 643–652.

  van Baaren, R. B., et al. (2004). Mimicry and prosocial behavior. Psychological Science, 15, 71–74.

  van der Vaart, E., et al. (2012). Corvid re-caching without ‘theory of mind’: A model. PLOS One, 7, e32904.

  van Schaik, C. P., et al. (2003). Orangutan cultures and the evolution of material culture. Science, 299, 102–105.

  van Wolkenten, M., et al. (2007). Inequity responses of monkeys modified by effort. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 18854–18859.

  Varki, A., et al. (1998). Great ape phenome project? Science, 282, 239–240.

  Vidal, G. (1981). Creation. New York: Doubleday.

  Villa, P., & Lenoir, M. (2009). Hunting and Hunting Weapons of the Lower and Middle Paleolithic of Europe. In J. J. Hublin & M. P. Richard (Eds.), Evolution of hominin diets: Integrating approaches to the study of palaeolithic subsistence (pp. 59–85). Dordrecht: Springer.

 

‹ Prev