The Wandering Mind

Home > Other > The Wandering Mind > Page 14
The Wandering Mind Page 14

by Michael C Corballis


  As we all know from attending a boring lecture, trying to listen to a symphony after a heavy meal, or simply trying to sleep on the plane, the mind can wander quite freely without the influence of drugs. Even undirected wandering can stimulate creativity indirectly, through what is known as ‘incubation’, in which ideas are developed while one is thinking of something else. This has even been demonstrated experimentally. People were given the task of inventing unusual uses for familiar objects, a task commonly used as a measure of creativity. After working on this for a short time, most of them were given a break. During the break, some engaged in a task demanding of memory, some in an undemanding task, and some simply sat quietly without doing anything. When the creativity task resumed, those who performed the undemanding task performed best, probably because their minds wandered; other research has shown that undemanding tasks are most likely to induce mind-wandering, more likely even than doing nothing. If you’re seeking inspiration, it seems a good idea to take a break and do something undemanding, like washing the dishes or watching a light TV show. Or perhaps knitting, which could explain why Agatha’s Christie’s Miss Marple, a compulsive knitter, was able to solve murder mysteries. Maybe Agatha Christie was herself a compulsive knitter, which is why she was able to create the murder mysteries in the first place.

  An unnamed physicist is said to have told the German psychologist Wolfgang Köhler: ‘We often talk about the three Bs, the Bus, the Bath, and the Bed. That is where the great discoveries are made in our science.’4 He or she was perhaps alluding to Poincaré’s mathematical inspiration that came to him as he stepped onto a bus, as well as to Archimedes’ famous ‘Eureka!’ discovery that the water rose when he was in the bath. As for the bed, dreams can sometimes lead to creative moments, but inspiration is perhaps more likely when we can’t sleep, instead allowing our minds to wander while we’re conscious enough to snare any insights. Perhaps one could add a fourth B, the boardroom, which provides an almost perfect environment for creative mind-wandering and incubation. And then there’s boredom itself. The Nobel Prizewinning poet Joseph Brodsky once declared: ‘Boredom is your window on the properties of time that one tends to ignore to the likely peril of one’s mental equilibrium. It is your window on time’s infinity. Once this window opens, don’t try to shut it; on the contrary, throw it wide open.’

  However you choose to wander, do not be discouraged into thinking that it is a waste of time. Of course, teacher was not always wrong—there are occasions when we need to attend in order to learn or finish some job. But nature also designed us to dream, to escape the channels that confine us. Remember from Chapter 1 the study by Jonathan Schooler and his associates on the frequency with which people zoned out while reading War and Peace? Well, those whose minds wandered most scored best on various measures of creativity. If the teacher or the board chairman catches you looking out the window when important matters are under discussion, you can explain that you are simply opening the doors of creativity.

  And if your mind occasionally wandered while reading this book, I hope it took you into territory that was stimulating, creative—and above all happy.

  References

  Chapter 1: Meandering Brain, Wandering Mind

  Epel, E. S., Puterman, E., Lin, J., Blackburn, E., Lazaro, A. and Mendez, W. B. (2013). ‘Wandering minds and aging cells’. Clinical Psychological Science, 1, 75–83.

  Ingvar, D. H. (1979). ‘“Hyperfrontal” distribution of the cerebral grey matter flow in resting wakefulness: On the functional anatomy of the conscious state’. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 60, 12–25.

  ——. (1985). ‘“Memory of the future”: An essay on the temporal organization of conscious awareness’. Human Neurobiology, 4, 127–136.

  Killingsworth, M. A. and Gilbert, D. T. (2010). ‘A wandering mind is an unhappy mind’. Science, 330, 932–932.

  Ottaviani, C. and Couyoumdjian, A. (2013). ‘Pros and cons of a wandering mind: A prospective study’. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article 524.

  Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A. and Shulman, G. L. (2001). ‘A default mode of brain function’. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 98, 676–682.

  Schooler, J. W., Reichle, E. D. and Halpern, D. V. (2005). ‘Zoning-out during reading: Evidence for dissociations between experience and meta-consciousness’. In D. T. Levin (ed.), Thinking and Seeing: Visual Metacognition in Adults and Children (pp. 204–226). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  Subramaniam, K. and Vinogradov, S. (2013). ‘Improving the neural mechanisms of cognition through the pursuit of happiness’. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, Article 452.

  Chapter 2: Memory

  Corkin, S. (2002). ‘What’s new with the amnesic patient H.M.?’. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 453–460.

  ——. (2013). Permanent Present Tense: The Man With No Memory, and What He Taught the World. London: Allen Lane.

  Kundera, M. (2002). Ignorance. New York: HarperCollins (translated from the French by L. Asher).

  Loftus, E. and Ketcham, K. (1994). The Myth of Repressed Memory: False Memories and Allegations of Sexual Abuse. New York: St. Martin’s Press.

  Luria, A. R. (1968). The Mind of a Mnemonist: A Little Book about a Vast Memory. London: Basic Books.

  Martin, V. C., Schacter, D. L., Corballis, M. C. and Addis, D. R. (2011). ‘A role for the hippocampus in encoding simulations of future events’. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 108, 13858–13863.

  Nabokov, V. (2000). Speak, Memory. London: Penguin Books.

  Ogden, J. A. (2012). Trouble in Mind: Stories from a Neuropsychologist’s Casebook. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  Raz, A., Packard, M. G., Alexander, G. M., Buhle, J. T., Zhu, H., Yu, S. and Peterson, B. S. (2009). ‘A slice of : An exploratory neuroimaging study of digit encoding and retrieval in a superior memorist’. Neurocase, 15, 361–372.

  Sacks, O. (1985). The Man Who Mistook His Wife for a Hat and other Clinical Tales. New York: Simon & Schuster.

  Spence, J. (1984). The Memory Palace of Matteo Ricci. London: Faber & Faber.

  Tammet, D. (2009). Embracing the Wide Sky. New York: Free Press.

  Treffert, D. A. and Christensen, D. D. (2006). ‘Inside the mind of a savant’. Scientific American Mind, 17, 55–55.

  von Hippel, W. and Trivers, R. (2011). ‘The evolution and psychology of self-deception’. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34, 1–56.

  Chapter 3: On Time

  Clayton, N. S., Bussey, T. J. and Dickinson, A. (2003). ‘Can animals recall the past and plan for the future?’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 685–691.

  Darwin, C. (1896). The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex (2nd edition). New York: Appleton.

  Markus, H. and Nurius, P. (1986). ‘Possible selves’. American Psychologist, 41, 954–969.

  Osvath, M. and Karvonen, E. (2012). ‘Spontaneous innovation for future deception in a male chimpanzee’. PLOS ONE, 7, e36782.

  Suddendorf, T. and Corballis, M. C. (2007). ‘The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans?’. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 299–351.

  Suddendorf, T. and Redshaw, J. (2013). ‘The development of mental scenario building and episodic foresight’. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1296, 135–153.

  Tulving, E. (1985). ‘Memory and consciousness’. Canadian Psychologist, 26, 1–12.

  Wearing, D. (2005). Forever Today: A Memoir of Love and Amnesia. New York: Doubleday.

  Chapter 4: The Hippo in the Brain

  Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T. and Schacter D. L. (2007). ‘Remembering the past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration’. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1363–1377.

  Corballis, M. C. (2013). ‘Mental time travel: The case for evolutionary continuity’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 5–6.

  Dalla Barba, G. and La Corte, V. (2013). ‘The hippocampus
, a time machine that makes errors’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 102–104.

  Ekstrom, A. D., Kahana, M. J., Caplan, J. B., Fields, T. A., Isham, E. A., Newman, E. L. and Fried, I. (2003). ‘Cellular networks underlying human spatial navigation’. Nature, 425, 184–187.

  Gross, C. G. (1993). ‘Huxley versus Owen: The hippocampus minor and evolution’. Trends in Neurosciences, 16, 493–498.

  Macphail, E. M. (2002). ‘The role of the avian hippocampus in spatial memory’. Psicologica, 23, 93–108.

  Maguire, E. A., Woollett, K. and Spiers, H. J. (2006). ‘London taxi drivers and bus drivers: A structural MRI and neuropsychological analysis’. Hippocampus, 16, 1091–1101.

  Milivojevic, B. and Doeller, C. F. (2013). ‘Mnemonic networks in the hippocampal formation: From spatial maps to temporal and conceptual codes’. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1037/a0033746.

  O’Keefe, J. and Nadel, L. (1978). The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

  Pastalkova, E., Itskov, V., Amarasingham, A. and Buzsáki, G. (2008). ‘Internally generated cell assembly sequences in the rat hippocampus’. Science, 321, 1322–1327.

  Smith, D. M. and Mizumori, S. J. Y. (2006). ‘Hippocampal place cells, context, and episodic memory’. Hippocampus, 16, 716–729.

  Suddendorf, T. (2013). ‘Mental time travel: continuities and discontinuities’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 151–152.

  Chapter 5: Wandering into other Minds

  Bloom, P. (2004). Descartes’ Baby: How the Science of Child Development Explains What Makes Us Human. New York: Basic Books.

  Call, J. and Tomasello, M. (2008). ‘Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 187–192.

  Darwin, C. (1872). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. London: John Murray.

  de Waal, F. B. M. (2012). ‘The antiquity of empathy’. Science, 336, 874–876.

  Grandin, T. and Johnson, C. (2005). Animals in Translation: Using the Mysteries of Autism to Decode Animal Behavior. New York: Scribner.

  Hare, B. and Woods, V. (2013). The Genius of Dogs: How Dogs are Smarter than You Think. London: Oneworld Publications.

  Humphrey, N. (1976). ‘The social function of intellect’. In P. P. G. Bateson and R. A. Hinde (eds), Growing Points in Ethology (pp. 303–317). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

  Kovács, A. M., Téglás, E. and Endress, A. D. (2011). ‘The social sense: Susceptibility to others’ beliefs in human infants and adults’. Science, 330, 1830–1834.

  Laing, R. D. (1970). Knots. London: Penguin.

  Marks, D. F. and Kammann, R. (1980). The Psychology of the Psychic. Buffalo, NY: Prometheus Books.

  Premack, D. and Woodruff, G. (1978). ‘Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind?’. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515–526.

  Radin, D. I. (2006). Entangled Minds: Extrasensory Experiences in a Quantum Reality. New York: Paraview Pocket Books.

  Randi, J. (1982). The Truth About Uri Geller. New York: Prometheus Books.

  Suddendorf, T. (2013). The Gap: The Science of What Separates Us from Other Animals. New York: Basic Books.

  Suddendorf, T. and Corballis, M. C. (1997). ‘Mental time travel and the evolution of the human mind’. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 123, 133–167.

  Taylor, M. (1999). Imaginary Companions and the Children Who Create Them. New York: Oxford University Press.

  Whiten, A. and Byrne, R. W. (1988). ‘Tactical deception in primates’. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 233–273.

  Wimmer, H. and Perner, J. (1983). ‘Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception’. Cognition, 13, 103–128.

  Chapter 6: Stories

  Abrahams, R. D. (1970). Deep Down in the Jungle: Negro Narrative Folklore from the Streets of Philadelphia. Chicago: Aldine.

  Bateson, G. (1982). ‘Difference, double description and the interactive designation of self’. In F. Allan Hanson (ed.), Studies in Symbolic and Cultural Communication (pp. 3–8). University of Kansas Publications in Anthropology No. 14. Lawrence: University of Kansas Press.

  Boyd, B. (2009). On the Origin of Stories: Evolution, Cognition, and Fiction. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

  Corballis, M. C. (2002). From Hand to Mouth: The Origins of Language. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

  Dunbar, R. I. M. (1998). Grooming, Gossip, and the Evolution of Language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  Engel, S. (1995). The Stories Children Tell: Making Sense of the Narratives of Childhood. New York: W. H. Freeman.

  Janet, P. (1928). L’Évolution de la mémoire et de la notion du temps: Leçons au Collège de France 1927–1928. Paris: Chahine.

  Kidd, D. C. and Castano, E. (2013). ‘Reading literary fiction improves theory of mind’. Science, 342, 377–380.

  Mechling, J. (1988). ‘“Banana cannon” and other folk traditions between humans and nonhuman animals’. Western Folklore, 48, 312–323.

  Niles, J. D. (2010). Homo Narrans: The Poetics and Anthropology of Oral Literature. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

  Salmond, A. (1975). ‘Mana makes the man: A look at Maori oratory and politics’. In M. Bloch (ed.), Political Language and Oratory in Traditional Society (pp. 45–63). New York: Academic Press.

  Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Shanker, S. G. and Taylor, T. J. (1998). Apes, Language, and the Human Mind. New York: Oxford University Press.

  Sugiyama, M. S. (2011). ‘The forager oral tradition and the evolution of prolonged juvenility’. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, Article 133.

  Thompson, T. (2011). ‘The ape that captured time: Folklore, narrative, and the human-animal divide’. Western Folklore, 69, 395–420.

  Trinkaus, E. (2011). ‘Late Pleistocene adult mortality patterns and modern human establishment’. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 108, 1267–1271.

  Turton, D. (1975). ‘The relationship between oratory and the emergence of influence among the Mursi’. In M. Bloch (ed.), Political Language and Oratory in Traditional Society. New York: Academic Press.

  Chapter 7: Tigers in the Night

  Darwin, C. (1872). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. London: John Murray.

  Foulkes, D. (1999). Children’s Dreaming and the Development of Consciousness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  Fox, K. C. R., Nijeboer, S., Solomonova, E., Domhoff, G. W. and Christoff, K. (2013). ‘Dreaming as mind wandering: Evidence from functional neuroimaging and first-person content reports’. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, Article 412.

  Freud, S. (1900). The Interpretation of Dreams. New York: Macmillan.

  Hobson, J. A. (2009). ‘REM sleep and dreaming: Towards a theory of protoconsciousness’. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 803–813.

  Honikawa, T., Tamaki, M., Miyawaki, Y. and Kamitani, Y. (2013). ‘Neural decoding of visual imagery during sleep’. Science, 340, 630–642.

  Revonsuo, A. (2000). ‘The reinterpretation of dreams: An evolutionary hypothesis of the function of dreaming’. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 877–901.

  Saurat, M.-T., Agbakou, M., Attigui, P., Golmard, J.-L. and Arnulf, I. (2011). ‘Walking dreams in congenital and acquired paraplegia’. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 1425–1432.

  Valli, K. and Revonsuo, A. (2009). ‘The threat simulation theory in light of recent empirical evidence: A review’. American Journal of Psychology, 122, 17–38.

  Wamsley, E. J. and Stickgold, R. (2010). ‘Dreaming and offline memory processing’. Current Biology, 20(23), R1010.

  Chapter 8: Hallucinations

  James, W. (1902). The Varieties of Religious Experience: A Study in Human Nature. London: Longmans, Green & Co.

  Jaynes, J. (1976). The Origin of Consciousness in the Breakdown of the Bicameral Mind. New York: Houghton Mifflin.

&n
bsp; McGilchrist, I. (2009). The Master and his Emissary: The Divided Brain and the Making of the Western World. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

  Penfield, W. and Perot, P. (1963). ‘The brain’s record of auditory and visual experience’. Brain, 86, 596–696.

  Rosenhan, D. L. (1973). ‘On being sane in insane places’. Science, 179, 250–258.

  Sacks, O. (2012). Hallucinations. New York: Random House.

  Sireteanu, R., Oertel, V., Morh, H., Linden, D. and Singer, W. (2008). ‘Graphical illustration and functional neuroimaging of visual hallucinations during prolonged blindfolding’. Perception, 37, 1805–1821.

  Vitorovic, D. and Biller, J. (2013). ‘Musical hallucinations and forgotten tunes—case report and brief literature review’. Frontiers in Neurology, 4, Article 109.

  Waugh, E. (1957). The Ordeal of Gilbert Pinfold. London: Chapman & Hall.

  Zubek, J. P. (ed.) (1969). Sensory Deprivation: Fifteen Years of Research. New York: Meredith.

  Chapter 9: The Creativity of the Wandering Mind

  Baird, B., Smallwood, J., Mrazek, M. D., Kam, J. W. Y., Franklin, M. S. and Schooler, J. W. (2012). ‘Inspired by distraction: Mind wandering facilitates creative incubation’. Psychological Science, 23, 1117–1122.

  Campbell, D. T. (1960). ‘Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes’. Psychological Review, 67, 380–400.

  Corballis, M. C. (1999). ‘Are we in our right minds?’. In S. Della Sala (ed.), Mind Myths: Exploring Popular Assumptions About the Mind and Brain (pp. 25–42). Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.

 

‹ Prev