Book Read Free

Elephant Sense and Sensibility

Page 9

by Michael Garstang


  52 Elephant Sense and Sensibility

  expressing grief and was deeply aware of who it was that she was touching and

  holding in her trunk. Not only was her behavior clear evidence of grief, but it

  also showed that she perceived the depth of the relationship between who her

  calf was and herself. Knobnose finally mated again, bore a healthy calf, re-

  gained her equilibrium, and returned to lead her herd once again.

  Numerous observers, including Darwin, believe that under extreme condi-

  tions of stress, elephants can break down and weep, shedding tears and uttering

  cries (see also Chapter 7).

  Elephants visit the remains of their kind and have been shown (McComb

  et al., 2001) to recognize the remains of elephants among the bones of other ani-

  mals. In contrast to the account above, McComb found that there seemed to be

  no particular recognition of kin, although when tusks were among the remains,

  these drew special attention. She also found that elephants can distinguish the

  bones of other elephants from those of large mammals and other nonelephant

  remains.

  Joyce Poole describes an incident involving Eleanor, one of the orphans

  raised by Daphne Sheldrick. A woman wearing an ivory bracelet was warned to

  hide the bracelet behind her back. She did so but Eleanor reached around behind

  her, took her hand in her trunk, and raised it up close to her eye to look at it.

  van Graan (cited in The Game Rangers Jan Roderigues, p. 56, 1992, per-

  mission granted by Roderigues), in dealing with crop-raiding elephants on the

  border of the Kruger National Park, had to shoot one of the bulls. The carcass of

  this bull was dismembered for later use. However, as night descended, the feet,

  tusks, trunk, and upper ear were moved and placed on an embankment some

  distance from the carcass.

  On return, shortly after sunrise the next day, there was no sign of the dis-

  membered parts. Numerous tracks of elephants did, however, mark the spot

  where the body parts were left. A search for these parts found them all back

  in a neat pile beside the carcass of the elephant. They, and the carcass, were

  partly covered with soil and vegetation. There were furrows in the soil where the

  ground had been disturbed with clear prints of the feet of the elephants who had

  done the work. Once again, there is clear recognition of who the dead elephant

  is, that his being has been disturbed and what belongs to him violated. It is

  more challenging to speculate on what motivated the other elephants to cover

  the body and the dismembered body parts. Reports of such behavior, however,

  are not uncommon (see, for example, the case of birth earlier in this chapter).

  It is difficult not to conclude that elephants are aware of the distress of oth-

  ers, empathizing with vocalization and other responses to their distress and at-

  tempting to alleviate the suffering of others or restore others to a better state.

  When this fails, they identify with the remains and may even treat them with

  what could be called respect.

  Elephants certainly recognize suffering and stress in other elephants and

  respond in ways that attempt to relieve such conditions. This response is not

  Empathy and Altruism Chapter | 8 53

  restricted to kin but is a generalized response to suffering and death of conspe-

  cifics. That elephants should show such a response is almost certainly related

  to their highly social structure. The death of the matriarch Eleanor led immedi-

  ately to the death of her calf and the loss of her knowledge to that family unit.

  In a more generalized sense, altruistic behavior exhibited to kin promotes the

  survival of the group. In evolutionary terms, natural selection of a beneficial

  trait such as empathy or altruism within the gene pool of the family benefits

  survival (Douglas-Hamilton et al., 2006; McComb et al., 2001).

  More rarely elephants have been reported to respond to other species includ-

  ing humans in distress. A nearly blind elderly woman returning to her village

  was overtaken by nightfall. She was too frail to climb into a tree and resorted to

  sitting with her back against the trunk of a large tree. Elephants arrived at this

  spot during the night and, finding the woman, covered her with branches and

  vegetation and remained around her for the duration of the night.

  More recently, Hutto (2014), in Touching the Wild: Living with the Mule

  Deer of Deadman Gulch, reports what he believes to be clear recognition by a

  mule deer of the death of her fawn and even the deaths of others within a much

  wider group of mule deer. Observing behavior of animals requires, as is demon-

  strated by Hutto, both in the case of wild turkeys (Hutto, 2006) and mule deer

  (Hutto, 2014), a significant commitment of effort and time.

  The effort by Bates and her colleagues to examine elephant empathy under

  controlled conditions in the wild points the way to how a significant part of

  the research required to understand elephant cognition must be done. Failure

  to place the elephant in its natural environment is likely to seriously bias if not

  invalidate most experiments. The role played by elephant society and the full

  spectrum of the environment prohibits acceptable design of experiments under

  most artificial circumstances.

  Experiments such as the mirror recognition test may be as invalid for

  elephants as they would be for bats. We as scientists are further heavily influ-

  enced by the need for rigor, recognizing the weakness of findings that cannot be

  quantified and our tendency to believe in the reductionist approach. Elephants,

  in particular, are likely to depend on multiple sensory inputs, are able to assimi-

  late such multiple inputs, and deal with feedback between them resulting in a

  response difficult to comprehend by humans that think primarily in terms of a

  single sensory system: sight.

  As a cautionary note to an earlier remark, Michael Finkel, in the July 2013

  issue of National Geographic magazine, reported on Daniel Kirk who had lost

  both eyes to retinal cancer at the age of 13 months. Daniel, at the age of 47,

  has taught himself to navigate by echo location. He produces clicking sounds,

  sometimes as fast as twice per second, which not only allow him to create im-

  ages in his mind of objects as much as 30 m (100 ft) to 45 m (150 ft) away, but to

  ride a bicycle on a city street. Close to 1000 blind students in over 30 countries

  have been taught to use echolocation.

  Chapter 9

  Communication

  Communication, especially in a highly social animal such as an elephant,

  may have played a significant role in their evolution and in natural selection.

  Animal communication favors callers whose vocalizations benefit their listen-

  ers (Seyfarth and Cheney, 2003b). As is described here, elephants vocalize most

  often in the presence of other elephants, emphasizing the social function of

  communication. Although signalers may vocalize to change a listener’s behav-

  ior, there is no general acceptance by researchers that animals call to inform oth-

  ers. The concensus amongst psychologists is that listeners acquire information

  from signals mainly by eavesdropping and not because the signaler intends tor />
  provide such information (Seyfarth and Cheney, 2003b). Dawkins (1989, p. 57)

  and Dawkins and Krebs (1978) believe that all animal communication repre-

  sents manipulation of the signal-receiver by the signal-sender.

  We show here and elsewhere that observed elephant behavior would suggest

  that such a view may not be entirely valid. Similarly, there is fairly universal

  agreement that the cognitive limitations of animals are responsible for the per-

  ceived differences in animal communication and human language. While the

  reach and extent of human language goes far beyond that of elephants, elements

  of the understanding of an elephant receiving a signal of the signaler’s intent are

  becoming increasingly apparent. The ability of elephants to recognize the men-

  tal states of other elephants and to vocalize with the specific intent of informing

  others and transmitting to those listeners knowledge that the signaler possesses

  may equally well exist.

  Individual recognition is central to social life. Elephants have knowledge of

  members that are within their own family group as well as in the wider popula-

  tion. This recognition, however, is primarily in the form of sound and smell and

  not sight. McComb et al. (2003) show that adult female elephants are familiar

  with and know the vocal identity of 14 families within the population, totaling

  some 100 individuals. While the full range of low-frequency calling patterns of

  elephants in the wild is unknown, it is likely that adult females maintain near-

  continuous contact via these calls. Older females in the herd have the most ex-

  tensive knowledge of the calls and identities of other elephants as well as sounds

  emanating from other sources. Processing and storage of this range of auditory

  input requires considerable cognitive ability.

  Elephant Sense and Sensibility. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-802217-7.00009-0

  Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

  55

  56 Elephant Sense and Sensibility

  Elephants generate and can detect sound over the widest range of fre-

  quencies of all mammals (see http://people.eku.edu/ritchisong/RITCHISO/

  infrasounddiagram.gif). The female Asian elephant tested by Heffner and Heffner (1982, 1984) was able to detect a 60 dB signal as low as 17 Hz and as high as

  10.5 kHz, which is the widest range known for any nonhuman mammal tested. In

  comparison, the range of human hearing is 20 Hz to 20 kHz (Soltis, 2010).

  SOUND GENERATION

  Sounds generated by vertebrates depend on lung capacity and the mass, length,

  and elasticity of the vocal folds in the larynx. These fundamental sounds are

  then modulated as they pass through and emerge from the passageways that

  constitute the vocal tract. The vocal tract acts as a filter and operates indepen-

  dently of the source (Fitch and Hauser, 2002).

  This independence between the “source” and the “filter” is considered to be

  the best current working hypothesis of how animals produce sound (Fitch and

  Hauser, 2002). However, much of what is known about the relationship between

  the source and filter has been learned from the study of humans, nonhuman pri-

  mates, and other animals such as species of deer (Fitch, 2000; Fitch and Hauser,

  2002; Fitch and Reby, 2001; Reby and McComb, 2003; Titze, 1994; Willmer

  et al., 2000; Wilson et al., 2001). Little is known of the source-filter relationship

  in elephants (Reby and McComb, 2003), yet it is useful to view the production

  of low-frequency elephant calls in these terms (Garstang, 2004).

  Air driven from the lungs sets the vocal folds in the larynx in motion. With

  their own elasticity and mass, these folds, responding to the air flow over them,

  act as mechanical vibrators that can generate self-oscillation (Fitch and Hauser,

  2002; Titze, 1994). When the folds close to the appropriate “phonatory” posi-

  tion, they generate acoustic energy. The period and thus the frequency of the

  opening and closing of the vocal folds produces the fundamental frequency

  (FO). This frequency is set passively by muscle tension, mass of the vocal

  cords, and lung pressure. There are small nonlinear oscillations around this

  fundamental frequency. These nonlinearities in the periodic vocal production

  provide structure to the morphology of the call and have been described in

  terms of deterministic chaos (Reby and McComb, 2003). Because the length,

  mass, and elasticity of the vocal folds are related to body size, the FO can

  be related to body size. However, these parameters (length, mass, elasticity)

  can change (e.g., with age) and the relationship between FO and body size is

  not robust (Fitch, 1997; McComb, 1991; Reby and McComb, 2003; Rendall,

  1996; Riede and Fitch, 1999). The inverse relationship between the length

  and mass of an elephant’s vocal folds predicts that it is capable of producing

  lower-frequency sounds than any other terrestrial animal. The prediction that

  larger animals produce lower frequencies than smaller animals of the same

  species has not been well verified by observations (McComb, 1991; Reby and

  McComb, 2003).

  Communication Chapter | 9 57

  The supralaryngeal vocal tract of the elephant is the respiratory tract from

  the larynx to the tip of the trunk. For the elephant the pharyngeal pouch, nasal

  cavity, membrane near the tip of the trunk, the highly mobile tip of the trunk

  and the length and ability to change the length of the trunk are, in combination,

  unique Elephantidae features (Garstang, 2004) (Figures 9.1 and 9.2). Their individual and collective role in controlling the air column in the vocal tract has

  not been carefully studied.

  Recent work, however, at the University of Vienna on an excised larynx of

  a 25-year-old female African elephant has both confirmed earlier supposition

  and described a number of new phenomena within the elephant vocal anatomy

  including the generation of vibrations of the vestibular folds, which increased

  the sound pressure levels by 12 dB. The study also showed that the anatomy of

  the elephant’s larynx is more complex than that of a human and is capable of

  producing multiple wave patterns (traveling, standing, and irregular vocal fold

  vibrations) (Herbst et al., 2013).

  The air column in the vocal tract has elasticity and mass that will vibrate

  preferentially at certain frequencies termed normal modes or resonances.

  In the simplest terms this column of air acts as though it is in a tube closed

  at one end such that the length of the tube is one-quarter of the wavelength.

  Wavelength is directly related to the speed of sound and inversely related to

  FIGURE 9.1 The vocal tract of an adult elephant including the trunk, the nasal cavity seen as a bump on the forehead, the pharyngeal cavity, and the larynx can amount to a length of nearly 4.4 m (15 ft). Both the exterior trunk and the larynx can be extended by another foot.

  58 Elephant Sense and Sensibility

  FIGURE 9.2 A cavity in the throat of the elephant called the pharyngeal pouch can be filled with water (up to 4 or 5 L) and used as an emergency drinking or cooling supply of water and influence the sound emerging from the vocal tract of the animal. (Pen and ink watercolor on wood by author.) the frequency. Thus, for the average speed of sound in the atmosphere near the

 
ground of 350 m (1148 ft) per second and a frequency of 20 Hz, the wavelength

  is 350/20 m = 17.5 m (57 ft), and the vocal tract length would be (1/4)(17.5 m) or

  4.4 m (15 ft). As we have seen, this is a realistic length of the vocal tract for an

  adult African elephant.

  The vocal tract will act as a filter depending on the transit time of the sound

  waves up (and down) the column. The speed of sound in this tract will govern

  the transit time and thus will depend on the composition of gases in the tube and

  the temperature of those gases (Pierce, 1981).

  The filter will act upon all of the frequencies being generated in the larynx

  (i.e., the FO and the nonlinear oscillations about the FO). The filter will thus shape

  the final form of the vocal signal. Prominent in this ultimate form of the signal

  will be “formants.” These formants are selectively amplified parts of the vocal

  signal, clearly visible in the sonogram of the call of an elephant as nearly equally

  spaced bands of acoustic energy (Figure 9.3). Embedded in this signal envelope but independent of the formants is the FO and the integer harmonics of the FO.

  The vocal tract length governs formant spacing. Formant spacing is a better

  predictor to body size than the size of the vocal folds or larynx (Fitch, 2000;

  Fitch and Hauser, 2002; Reby and McComb, 2003). In addition to changing

  the length of the vocal tract by extending its trunk, the elephant may be able to

  elongate the vocal tract by contraction of the larynx or laryngeal descent (Fitch

  and Reby, 2001; Reby and McComb, 2003). The presence or absence of water

  in the pharyngeal pouch may also influence vocal tract length. The function of

  the narrow connective strip of tissue dividing the lower part of the trunk into two

  Communication Chapter | 9 59

  500

  400

  300

  F4

  Frequency (Hz) 200

  F3

  F2

  100

  F1

  F0

  0

  0

  2

  4

  Time (s)

  FIGURE 9.3 Waveform of a female contact call showing the fundamental frequency (F0) and harmonics and the position of the first four formants (F1–F4). Based on Garstang (2004); from McComb et al. (2003).

 

‹ Prev