Book Read Free

Women Don't Ask

Page 17

by Linda Babcock


  for your family.”54 One of our society’s strongest gender norms for

  women, in contrast, is that they will be modest and selfless. As a result,

  many people don’t consider being preoccupied by money or attaching

  a dollar value to their work and time to be proper or attractive for a

  woman.55

  Linda Evangelista, one of the first models to be identified as a “su-

  permodel,” earned an avalanche of derision in the summer of 1990

  when she admitted to a reporter that she and Christy Turlington, an-

  other “supermodel,” had an expression they liked to use: “We don’t

  wake up for less than $10,000 a day.” Loudly denounced at the time,

  she has been dogged by the remark ever since. As recently as the Sep-

  tember 2001 edition of Vogue, an interviewer pressed her again to ex-

  plain her remark. Evangelista said, “I feel like those words are going to

  be engraved on my tombstone. . . . I apologized for it. I acknowledged

  it. . . . Would I hope that I would never say something like that ever

  again? Yes.” Keep in mind that Evangelista made this remark in 1990,

  after a decade (the 1980s) in which everyone from Donald Trump to

  Michael Milken boasted of his huge income on television talk shows,

  in the society pages, and in the financial news—a decade in which accu-

  mulating wealth and flaunting it amounted to a national obsession. But

  Evangelista’s story tells us that what is good for the gander is not good for the goose. When a woman knows what she’s worth—and feels

  proud of her abilities and of what she can earn—she sets herself up to

  be scorned and chastised.

  98

  S C A R I N G T H E B O Y S

  Caring more about relationships than about personal gain represents

  another powerful gender norm for women. The media’s treatment of an

  episode at the 2002 Winter Olympics in Salt Lake City provided an

  object lesson for women on the dangers of violating this norm. Jean

  Racine, considered the top female bobsled driver in the world and the

  Olympic front-runner for the American women’s bobsledding team,

  spent most of her career partnered with a friend (many media sources

  said her “best friend”), Jen Davidson. Racine and Davidson competed

  in the two-person version of the sport, in which one athlete, the driver,

  sits in front and steers while the other, the brakewoman, pushes from

  behind to get the sled started down the course and then stops the sled

  at the bottom. Brakewomen need to be very strong. Racine was the

  driver and Davidson the brakewoman until two months before the

  games. Then, feeling that Davidson was not as strong as another player,

  Racine switched partners—or, as the media reported it, “dumped” Da-

  vidson. Shortly before the games, Racine’s new partner, Gea Johnson,

  suffered a hamstring injury, and Racine tried switching partners again,

  this time asking a relative newcomer, Vonetta Flowers, to join her.

  Flowers turned Racine down and with her partner, Jill Bakken, eventu-

  ally won the gold medal. Racine and the injured Gea Johnson did not

  perform well and failed to win a medal.

  This story was widely covered, with everyone from the New York

  Times to USA Today to the supermarket tabloids and both network and cable news programs weighing in with their judgments. The reporting,

  for the most part, reduced this story of personal struggle, hard choices,

  and disappointment to the realm of soap opera, a trivial squabble

  among women, with even such august news bodies as NBC dubbing

  the episode “As the Sled Turns.” No one claimed that Jen Davidson was

  faster than the other brakewomen who made the U.S. team, and a few

  news sources even conceded that switching partners is extremely com-

  mon in the sport, among male bobsledders as well as female. Nonethe-

  less, press reports described Racine as “ruthless” and “without remorse,”

  referred to her behavior as “scandalous” and “appalling,” and implied

  that she deserved to lose because she had put her own interests above

  the claims of friendship. Flowers, on the other hand, deserved to win

  because she’d been loyal. “Perhaps warmth and sweetness have their

  place in the cutthroat world of Olympic bobsledding. Loyalty does, at

  least,” wrote the New York Times’s reporter.56

  The thing is: Jean Racine was an Olympic-caliber athlete. Like any

  athlete, her chances to compete in the Olympics were limited, and she

  99

  C H A P T E R 4

  wanted to win. That’s what the Olympics are about, after all. And she

  put her personal ambition and desire—she put what she wanted—

  ahead of relationship concerns, a major taboo for a woman. For this,

  she was publicly lambasted. The message to women: If trying to get

  what you want means violating gender norms for women, don’t do it.

  You may not get what you want, and on top of that disappointment

  you’ll be roundly criticized and publicly shamed.

  Faludi believes that men, and many women, combat their fear that

  masculinity is threatened by women’s success by trying to shift the “cul-

  tural gears” into reverse. They do this by promoting the idea that the

  movement of women into the workplace is responsible for many of

  society’s problems, especially those involving families and children.57

  So the media publishes stories with titles like “Feminism Is Bad for

  Women’s Health Care” (from the Wall Street Journal)58 and conservative thinkers produce books such as A Return to Modesty: Discovering the Lost

  Virtue, by Wendy Shalit59; Domestic Tranquility: A Brief against Feminism, by F. Carolyn Graglia60; and The War against Boys: How Misguided Feminism Is Harming Our Young Men, by Christina Hoff Sommers.61

  In demonizing feminists and telling women that they’re responsible

  for society’s problems, these reactionary forces teach businesses that it’s

  permissible to penalize women for asking to do jobs typically performed

  by men—or simply for pursuing their own professional goals rather

  than deferring to the needs and ambitions of others. They can also make

  women feel less sure that it’s okay for them to want what they want,

  especially if what they want involves professional success. This can per-

  suade them to scale back their ambitions and to hope for—and ask

  for—fewer of life’s rewards.

  Women Have Learned Their Lessons Well

  The oppressive but inescapable message—that women will be punished

  for exceeding the bounds of acceptable behavior—has come through

  loud and clear, and women have adapted their behavior accordingly.62

  Ariadne, 33, is an MBA who enjoyed a successful career in public rela-

  tions before becoming a full-time mother. Ariadne has a very direct

  manner. Although she believes that a similarly direct man would be

  perceived as a “straight-shooter” or a “no-nonsense guy,” her style has

  prompted people to call her a bitch or complain that she is too aggres-

  100

  S C A R I N G T H E B O Y S

  sive. As a result, Ariadne learned in the course of her career to tone

  down her personal style and adopt a less straightforward manner. She

  would even avoid claimi
ng credit for her own ideas (and asking for

  appropriate recognition) because she found that letting other people

  think her good ideas were their own helped get those ideas imple-

  mented, and backfired less on her.

  An extensive body of research confirms that Ariadne’s is not an iso-

  lated case: Women consistently adjust their behavior between private

  and public settings—revealing their clear understanding that they may

  pay a penalty for behaving freely when observed by others. Of course,

  both men and women behave differently in public than they do at

  home, but research shows that women adjust their behavior more. In

  one of the “pay allocation” experiments mentioned in chapter 2, for

  example, men and women were instructed to work on a task until they

  had “earned” four dollars. Although women worked longer and harder

  than men in the “private,” unobserved condition (22 percent longer),

  they worked even longer if the amount of time they worked was moni-

  tored by the experimenter (52 percent longer than men). Men did not

  work longer when they were observed.63 This tells us that women have

  learned that they must pay more attention than men to the impressions

  they make on others, presumably because they fear the penalties for

  counterstereotypical behavior.

  Other research confirms that women conform more to gender roles

  in public than in private. In one study, researchers asked college stu-

  dents to estimate their grade-point averages (GPAs) for the upcoming

  semester either privately, on paper, or out loud to a peer. Although

  there were no gender differences between the male and female students’

  predictions in the private condition, the female students’ estimates were

  lower in the presence of a peer (the males’ estimates did not change).64

  A review of the research in this area concludes that unlike men, women

  “often limit their displays of achievement-oriented behavior to situa-

  tions in which autonomy and privacy are assured.”65

  Women have learned, in other words, that asking through their ac-

  tions to be recognized for their abilities and accomplishments can be a

  mistake. This self-consciousness about being observed extends to nego-

  tiation contexts, in which women request lower salaries when another

  person is present than they request when they assume no one else is

  watching. Men’s requests, on the other hand, increase in the presence of another person.66

  101

  C H A P T E R 4

  Do Not Compete

  Women don’t just modify their behavior in public settings, one study

  suggests, they may also shy away from competition, especially competi-

  tion with men. For this study, three economists, Uri Gneezy, Muriel

  Niederle, and Aldo Rustichini, asked female and male engineering stu-

  dents to work through mazes on a computer. At first, the students

  worked on their own and were paid a flat rate for each maze they com-

  pleted (the “piece-rate” condition). In this situation, men and women

  completed the same number of mazes on average. Then the researchers

  asked the same students to participate in a “tournament,” in which three

  female students and three male students would compete to see who

  could complete the most mazes in a set amount of time. The winner

  would be paid six times as much for each maze solved as he or she had

  earned in the piece-rate condition, while the rest of the students would

  earn no money for their work.67

  Traditional economic theory would expect every participant to com-

  plete more mazes in the tournament condition than in the piece-rate

  condition because the reward for winning would give everyone an in-

  centive to work harder. But Gneezy, Niederle, and Rustichini found

  that this was true only of the men. Whereas men completed 34 percent

  more mazes during the tournaments than they’d solved in the piece-

  rate condition, the number of mazes the women solved did not increase.

  The men didn’t suddenly get smarter—the tournament setting inspired

  them to compete with each other and try harder. But the tournament

  did not have the same impact on the women.

  One might conclude from this study that women simply don’t like

  to compete. To explore this hypothesis, the researchers organized addi-

  tional tournaments in which they segregated the groups by gender.

  They found that the performance of the men in the all-male tourna-

  ments was identical to their performance in the mixed-gender tourna-

  ments: The incentive of “winning” prompted them to increase their ef-

  forts over the piece-rate condition by the same amount no matter who

  they were competing against. But the most revealing data emerged from

  the all-female tournaments: The women completed far more mazes in

  the all-female tournament groups than in either the piece-rate condition

  or in the tournaments in which they were competing against men.

  One explanation for these uneven results could be that women be-

  lieve that men are better at solving mazes than women. Assuming they

  102

  S C A R I N G T H E B O Y S

  won’t win in a mixed-gender tournament, they consequently don’t try.

  Or stereotype threat may play a part: If women believe that men are

  better at solving mazes, this could undermine their performance at a

  subconscious level. Although the authors could not rule out these

  hypotheses, we can find nothing to suggest that mazes, which involve

  pretty basic skills, are in fact gender-defined and perceived to be the

  province of men. Another possible conclusion is that women just don’t

  like competing against men. Much of what we know about gender

  norms supports this interpretation: Boys learn that they are expected to

  compete, that being a good competitor is a defining male trait. They

  also learn that they are expected to demonstrate superior ability over

  girls in certain areas (intelligence, physical prowess, business success)

  and that this superiority is central to our society’s definition of maleness.

  Girls also learn these lessons about males. Because negotiation contains

  within it a basic form of competition, both males and females in our

  culture may make the connection that this consequently cannot be a

  woman’s domain. To compete with a man in a negotiation and win—

  to get him to give you a better raise than he wanted, or a better price

  for a car, or more responsibility on a project than he intended—may

  threaten his socially received idea of his own maleness. And women

  learn that this is rarely a good idea, because such a destabilizing threat

  will almost inevitably rebound in negative ways, punishing the woman

  who posed it.

  She may pay a price in her private life as well as at work. In Creating

  A Life: Professional Women and the Quest for Children, the economist

  Sylvia Ann Hewlett reports that “the more successful the woman, the

  less likely it is she will find a husband or bear a child.”68 Although many

  men scoff at the notion that they feel threatened by smart women or

  are less likely to date them, this phenomenon seems to persist. Two

  f
emale Harvard MBA students interviewed on the television newsmaga-

  zine 60 Minutes in 2002 confessed that they no longer admit to men that they go to Harvard, because men feel too threatened by their success to

  pursue relationships with them.69

  The popular cable television series Sex and the City, about the per-

  sonal lives of four New York career women (one of whom quits working

  to get married), illustrated this dilemma in one episode.70 Miranda, one

  of the show’s four principal characters, is a successful lawyer and a

  partner in her firm. Having observed that her career success frightens

  off many of the men she meets, Miranda pretends that she is a flight

  attendant to see if men will respond to her differently. This fiction, to

  103

  C H A P T E R 4

  her chagrin, turns out to be very successful: Men respond to her far

  more enthusiastically than before, concisely demonstrating the pressure

  women feel to downplay their accomplishments in order to protect men

  from being intimidated—and to protect their own chances of establish-

  ing relationships with them.

  Marcela, the nuclear engineer, described how she learned this lesson.

  When she was growing up, she said, “girls being smart was definitely

  an issue; when you were in your dating years the whole thing was not

  to let the guys know how smart you were. Because if they ever found out

  that your SAT scores were a lot higher than theirs then they wouldn’t go

  out with you or whatever.” She also said, “There was a point at which

  . . . I was told that I shouldn’t be so obvious in my accomplishments.”

  This lesson influences Marcela’s professional behavior to this day. Peri-

  odically, she must write up an assessment of her abilities and accom-

  plishments as part of her firm’s “rating” process for awarding raises,

  bonuses, and promotions. Implicit in the process is the expectation that

  she will indicate what she feels she deserves for the work she has done—

  a form of asking. She hates doing this, she said, because she doesn’t

  like “the kind of exercise where you have to either write about your

  contributions or your accomplishments. . . . Not because I don’t think

  that I’ve accomplished anything or made contributions but because I

  don’t like writing it down. It just makes me uncomfortable to have to

  self-promote. I’m not very comfortable being self-promoting.”

 

‹ Prev