Sensation_The New Science of Physical Intelligence

Home > Other > Sensation_The New Science of Physical Intelligence > Page 22
Sensation_The New Science of Physical Intelligence Page 22

by Thalma Lobel


  Chapter 5. The Lady in Red: Red and Sexual Attraction

  1. A. J. Elliot and D. Niesta (2008). Romantic red: Red enhances men’s attraction to women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95 (5), 1150–64.

  2. A. J. Elliot, J. L. Tracy, A. D. Pazda, and A. T. Beall (2012). Red enhances women’s attractiveness to men: First evidence suggesting universality. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49 (1), 165–68.

  3. D. Niesta Kayser, A. J. Elliot, and R. Feltman (2010). Red and romantic behavior in men viewing women. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40 (6), 901–8.

  4. N. Guéguen and C. Jacob (2012). Clothing color and tipping: Gentlemen patrons give more tips to waitresses with red clothes. Journal of Hospitality and Tourism Research, April 18, 2012.

  5. N. Guéguen (2012). Color and women hitchhikers’ attractiveness: Gentlemen drivers preferred. Color Research and Application, 37 (1), 76–78.

  6. C. Waitt, M. S. Gerald, A. C. Little, and E. Kraiselburd (2006). Selective attention toward female secondary sexual color in male rhesus macaques. American Journal of Primatology, 68 (7), 738–44.

  7. J. M. Setchell and E. J. Wickings (2005). Dominance, status signals and coloration in male mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx). Ethology, 111 (1), 25–50.

  8. S. R. Pryke and S. C. Griffith (2006). Red dominates black: Agonistic signalling among head morphs in the colour polymorphic Gouldian finch. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 273 (1589), 949–57.

  9. I. C. Cuthill, S. Hunt, C. Cleary, and C. Clark (1997). Colour bands, dominance, and body mass regulation in male zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata). Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 264 (1384), 1093–99.

  10. T. C. Bakker and M. Milinski (1993). The advantages of being red: Sexual selection in the stickleback. Marine and Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology, 23 (1–4), 287–300.

  11. A. C. Little and R. A. Hill (2007). Attribution to red suggests special role in dominance signalling. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 5 (1–4), 161–68.

  12. A. J. Elliot, D. Niesta Kayser, T. Greitemeyer, S. Lichtenfeld, R. H. Gramzow, M. A. Maier, and H. Liu (2010). Red, rank, and romance in women viewing men. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139 (3), 399–417.

  13. N. M. Puccinelli, R. Chandrashekaran, D. Grewal, and R. Suri (2013). Are men seduced by red? The effect of red versus black prices on price perceptions. Journal of Retailing, 89 (2), 115–25.

  14. T. L. Morris, J. Gorham, S. H. Cohen, and D. Huffman (1996). Fashion in the classroom: Effects of attire on student perceptions of instructors in college classes. Communication Education, 45 (2), 135–48.

  15. P. Glick, S. Larsen, C. Johnson, and H. Branstiter (2005). Evaluations of sexy women in low- and high-status jobs. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 29 (4), 389–95.

  16. C. Y. Shao, J. Baker, and J. A. Wagner (2004). The effects of appropriateness of service contact personnel dress on customer expectations of service quality and purchase intention: The moderating influences of involvement and gender. Journal of Business Research, 57 (10), 1164–76.

  17. H. Adam and A. D. Galinsky (2012). Enclothed cognition. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48 (4), 918–25.

  Chapter 6. In Contrast: Separating the Light from the Darkness

  1. B. P. Meier, M. D. Robinson, and G. L. Clore (2004). Why good guys wear white: Automatic inferences about stimulus valence based on brightness. Psychological Science, 15 (2), 82–87.

  2. D. Lakens, G. R. Semin, and F. Foroni (2011). But for the bad, there would not be good: Grounding valence in brightness through shared relational structures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141 (3), 584–94.

  3. M. G. Frank and T. Gilovich (1988). The dark side of self- and social perception: Black uniforms and aggression in professional sports. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54 (1), 74–85.

  4. G. D. Webster, G. R. Urland, and J. Correll (2012). Can uniform color color aggression? Quasi-experimental evidence from professional ice hockey. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3 (3), 274–81.

  5. B. P. Meier, M. D. Robinson, L. E. Crawford, and W. J. Ahlvers (2007). When “light” and “dark” thoughts become light and dark responses: Affect biases brightness judgments. Emotion, 7 (2), 366–76.

  6. H. Song, A. J. Vonasch, B. P. Meier, and J. A. Bargh (2012). Brighten up: Smiles facilitate perceptual judgment of facial lightness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48 (1), 450–52.

  7. P. Banerjee, P. Chatterjee, and J. Sinha (2012). Is it light or dark? Recalling moral behavior changes perception of brightness. Psychological Science, 23 (4), 407–9.

  8. G. D. Sherman and G. L. Clore (2009). The color of sin: White and black are perceptual symbols of moral purity and pollution. Psychological Science, 20 (8), 1019–25.

  9. C. Zhong, V. K. Bohns, and F. Gino (2010). Good lamps are the best police: Darkness increases dishonesty and self-interested behavior. Psychological Science, 21 (3), 311–14.

  10. M. aan het Rot, D. S. Moskowitz, and S. N. Young. (2008). Exposure to bright light is associated with positive social interaction and good mood over short time periods: A naturalistic study in mildly seasonal people. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 42 (4), 311–19.

  11. S. Leppämäki, T. Partonen, P. Piiroinen, J. Haukka, and J. Lönnqvist (2003). Timed bright-light exposure and complaints related to shift work among women. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment and Health, 29 (1), 22–26.

  Chapter 7. Space, the Mental Frontier: Physical and Psychological Distance

  1. L. Festinger, S. Schachter, and K. Back (1950). Social pressures in informal groups: A study of human factors in housing. Oxford, England: Harper.

  2. D. P. Kennedy, J. Gläscher, J. M. Tyszka, & R. Adolphs (2009). Personal space regulation by the human amygdala. Nature Neuroscience, 12 (10), 1226[0].

  3. J. Xu, H. Shen, and R. S. Wyer (2012). Does the distance between us matter? Influences of physical proximity to others on consumer choice. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22 (3), 418–23.

  4. J. Mumm and B. Mutlu (2011). Human-robot proxemics: Physical and psychological distancing in human-robot interaction. 2011 Sixth ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction, 331–38.

  5. A. Galin, M. Gross, and G. Gosalker (2007). E-negotiation versus face-to-face negotiation: What has changed—if anything? Computers in Human Behavior, 23 (1), 787–97.

  6. L. E. Williams and J. A. Bargh (2008). Keeping one’s distance: The influence of spatial distance cues on affect and evaluation. Psychological Science, 19 (3), 302–8.

  7. E. M. Sahlstein (2004). Relating at a distance: Negotiating being together and being apart in long-distance relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 21 (5), 689–710.

  Chapter 8. High and Mighty: Vertical Position, Size, and Power

  1. T. W. Schubert (2005). Your highness: Vertical positions as perceptual symbols of power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89 (1), 1–21.

  2. K. Zanolie, S. v. Dantzig, I. Boot, J. Wijnen, T. W. Schubert, S. R. Giessner, and D. Pecher (2012). Mighty metaphors: Behavioral and ERP evidence that power shifts attention on a vertical dimension. Brain and Cognition, 78 (1), 50–58.

  3. T. W. Schubert. Your highness: Vertical positions as perceptual symbols of power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89 (1), 1–21.

  4. S. R. Giessner and T. W. Schubert (2007). High in the hierarchy: How vertical location and judgments of leaders’ power are interrelated. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 104 (1), 30–44.

  5. P. A. Higham and D. W. Carment. (1992). The rise and fall of politicians: The judged heights of Broadbent, Mulroney and Turner before and after the 1988 Canadian federal election. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue Canadienne des Sciences du Comportement, 24 (3), 404–9.

  6. S. R. Giessner and T. W. Schubert (2007). High in the hierarchy: How vertic
al location and judgments of leaders’ power are interrelated. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 104 (1), 30–44.

  7. V. Carrieri and M. De Paola (2012). Height and subjective well-being in Italy. Economics and Human Biology, 10 (3), 289–98.

  8. T. A. Judge and D. M. Cable (2004). The effect of physical height on workplace success and income: Preliminary test of a theoretical model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89 (3), 428–40.

  9. A. J. Yap, M. F. Mason, and D. R. Ames (2012). The powerful size others down: The link between power and estimates of others’ size. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 49 (3), 591–94.

  10. M. M. Duguid and J. A. Goncalo (2012). Living large. Psychological Science, 23 (1), 36–40.

  11. S. R. Giessner, M. K. Ryan, T. W. Schubert, and N. van Quaquebeke (2011). The power of pictures: Vertical picture angles in power pictures. Media Psychology, 14 (4), 442–64.

  12. B. P. Meier and S. Dionne. (2009). Downright sexy: Verticality, implicit power, and perceived physical attractiveness. Social Cognition, 27 (6), 883–92.

  13. Giessner, Ryan, Schubert, and van Quaquebeke. The power of pictures.

  14. B. P. Meier and M. D. Robinson (2004). Why the sunny side is up: Associations between affect and vertical position. Psychological Science, 15 (4), 243–47.

  15. B. P. Meier, D. J. Hauser, M. D. Robinson, C. K. Friesen, and K. Schjeldahl (2007). What’s “up” with God? Vertical space as a representation of the divine. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93 (5), 699–710.

  16. E. L. Brainerd (1994). Pufferfish inflation: Functional morphology of postcranial structures in Diodon holocanthus (Tetraodontiformes). Journal of Morphology, 220 (3), 243–61.

  17. L. F. Toledo, I. Sazima, and C. F. Haddad (2011). Behavioural defences of anurans: An overview. Ethology Ecology and Evolution, 23 (1), 1–25.

  18. H. W. Greene (1988). Antipredator mechanisms in reptiles, 1–152 In: C. Gans and R. B. Huey (eds.), Biology of the Reptilia, Vol. 16, Ecology B, Defense and life history. New York: Alan R. Liss.

  19. D. Goodwin (1956). Further observations on the behaviour of the jay Garrulus glandarius. Ibis, 98 (2), 186–219.

  20. F. De Waal (2007). Chimpanzee politics: Power and sex among apes. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.

  21. T. W. Schubert, S. Waldzus, and S. R. Giessner (2009). Control over the association of power and size. Social Cognition, 27 (1), 1–19.

  22. D. Dubois, D. D. Rucker, and A. D. Galinsky (2012). Super size me: Product as a signal of status. Journal of Consumer Research, 38 (6), 1047–62.

  23. D. R. Carney, A. J. C. Cuddy, and A. J. Yap (2010). Power posing. Psychological Science, 21 (10), 1363–68.

  Chapter 9. Out, Damned Spot: Guilt, Morality, and Cleaning

  1. C. B. Zhong and K. Liljenquist (2006). Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science, 313 (5792), 1451–52.

  2. N. Fairbrother, S. J. Newth, and S. Rachman (2005). Mental pollution: Feelings of dirtiness without physical contact. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43 (1), 121–30.

  3. V. Lee (2012). Hell du jour: Meet Israel’s daylight prostitutes. Haaretz, October 11, http://www.haaretz.com/weekend/magazine/hell-du-jour-meet-israels-daylight-prostitutes-1.469461.

  4. S. W. S. Lee and N. Schwarz (2010). Dirty hands and dirty mouths: Embodiment of the moral-purity metaphor is specific to the motor modality involved in moral transgression. Psychological Science, 21 (10), 1423–25.

  5. Zhong and Liljenquist. Washing away your sins.

  6. H. A. Chapman, D. A. Kim, J. M. Susskind, and A. K. Anderson (2009). In bad taste: Evidence for the oral origins of moral disgust. Science, 323 (5918), 1222–26.

  7. S. Schnall, J. Haidt, G. L. Clore, and A. H. Jordan (2008). Disgust as embodied moral judgment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34 (8), 1096–1109.

  8. K. J. Eskine, N. A. Kacinik, and J. J. Prinz (2011). A bad taste in the mouth: Gustatory disgust influences moral judgment. Psychological Science, 22 (3), 295–99.

  9. S. Schnall, J. Benton, and S. Harvey (2008). With a clean conscience: Cleanliness reduces the severity of moral judgments. Psychological Science, 19 (12), 1219–22.

  10. Ibid.

  11. C. Zhong, B. Strejcek, and N. Sivanathan (2010). A clean self can render harsh moral judgment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46 (5), 859–62.

  12. S. W. S. Lee and N. Schwarz (2010). Washing away postdecisional dissonance. Science, 328 (5979), 709.

  13. A. J. Xu, R. Zwick, and N. Schwarz (2012). Washing away your (good or bad) luck: Physical cleansing affects risk-taking behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141 (1), 26–30.

  Chapter 10. Sweet Smell of Success: Taste and Smell

  1. B. P. Meier, S. K. Moeller, M. Riemer-Peltz, and M. D. Robinson (2012). Sweet taste preferences and experiences predict prosocial inferences, personalities, and behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102 (1), 163–74.

  2. C. B. Zhong and S. E. DeVoe (2010). You are how you eat: Fast food and impatience. Psychological Science, 21 (5), 619–22.

  3. L. Douce and W. Janssens (2013). The presence of a pleasant ambient scent in a fashion store: The moderating role of shopping motivation and affect intensity. Environment and Behavior, 45 (2), 215–38.

  4. J. Chebat, M. Morrin, and D. Chebat (2009). Does age attenuate the impact of pleasant ambient scent on consumer response? Environment and Behavior, 41 (2), 258–67.

  5. E. R. Spangenberg, A. E. Crowley, and P. W. Henderson (1996). Improving the store environment: Do olfactory cues affect evaluations and behaviors? Journal of Marketing, 60 (2), 67–80.

  6. K. Ravn (2007). Smells like sales. Los Angeles Times, August 20, F-1.

  7. A. R. Hirsch (1995). Effects of ambient odors on slot-machine usage in a Las Vegas casino. Psychology and Marketing, 12 (7), 585–94.

  8. N. Guéguen and C. Petr (2006). Odors and consumer behavior in a restaurant. International Journal of Hospitality Management, 25 (2), 335–39.

  9. H. N. Schifferstein, K. S. Talke, and D. Oudshoorn (2011). Can ambient scent enhance the nightlife experience? Chemosensory Perception, 4 (1–2), 55–64.

  10. M. Moss, S. Hewitt, L. Moss, and K. Wesnes (2008). Modulation of cognitive performance and mood by aromas of peppermint and ylang-ylang. International Journal of Neuroscience, 118 (1), 59–77; P. R. Zoladz and B. Raudenbush (2005). Cognitive enhancement through stimulation of the chemical senses. North American Journal of Psychology, 7 (1), 125–38; S. Barker, P. Grayhem, J. Koon, J. Perkins, A. Whalen, and B. Raudenbush (2003). Improved performance on clerical tasks associated with administration of peppermint odor. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 97 (3), 1007–10; K. McCombs, B. Raudenbush, A. Bova, and M. Sappington (2011). Effects of peppermint scent administration on cognitive video game performance. North American Journal of Psychology, 13 (3), 383–90.

  11. B. Raudenbush (2000). The effects of odors on objective and subjective measures of physical performance. Aroma-Chology Review, 9 (1), 1–5.

  12. B. Raudenbush, N. Corley, and W. Eppich (2001). Enhancing athletic performance through the administration of peppermint odor. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 23 (2), 156–60.

  13. R. A. Baron (1997). The sweet smell of . . . helping: Effects of pleasant ambient fragrance on prosocial behavior in shopping malls. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23 (5), 498–503.

  14. N. Guéguen (2012). The sweet smell of . . . courtship: Effects of pleasant ambient fragrance on women’s receptivity to a man’s courtship request. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 32 (2), 123–25.

  15. D. M. Zemke and S. Shoemaker (2008). A sociable atmosphere: Ambient scent’s effect on social interaction. Cornell Hospitality Quarterly, 49 (3), 317–29.

  16. R. W. Holland, M. Hendriks, and H. Aarts (2005). Smells like clean spirit: Nonconscious effects of scent on cognition and behavior. Psychological Science, 16 (9), 689–93.

  17. S. W. Lee an
d N. Schwarz (2012). Bidirectionality, mediation, and moderation of metaphorical effects: The embodiment of social suspicion and fishy smells. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103 (5), 737–49.

  Chapter 11. Turning on Lights Outside the Box: Embodying Metaphors

  1. X. Li, L. Wei, and D. Soman (2010). Sealing the emotions genie: The effects of physical enclosure on psychological closure. Psychological Science, 21 (8), 1047–50.

  2. A. K. Leung, S. Kim, E. Polman, L. S. Ong, L. Qiu, J. A. Goncalo, and J. Sanchez-Burks (2012). Embodied metaphors and creative “acts.” Psychological Science, 23 (5), 502–9.

  3. S. Mednick (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69 (3), 220–32.

  4. J. J. Ratey (2010). Spark! The revolutionary new science of exercise and the brain. London: Quercus Books.

  5. M. L. Slepian, M. Weisbuch, A. M. Rutchick, L. S. Newman, and N. Ambady (2010). Shedding light on insight: Priming bright ideas. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46 (4), 696–700.

  6. G. M. Fitzsimons, T. L. Chartrand, and G. J. Fitzsimons (2008). Automatic effects of brand exposure on motivated behavior: How Apple makes you “think different.” Journal of Consumer Research, 35 (1), 21–35.

  7. E. B. Gurman (1989). Travel abroad: A way to increase creativity? Educational Research Quarterly, 13 (3), 12–16.

  Index

  A

  abstractions

  guilt and, 154–55

  and importance of embodied cognition theory, 212

  metaphorical link of sensations and, 193, 213

  metaphors as aid to understanding, 213

  physical activities and learning about, 203–4

  scaffolding between concrete concepts and, 163

  sensations as activating, 209–10

  texture and, 31, 32–33

 

‹ Prev