Data Versus Democracy
Page 5
propaganda problem.
C H A P T E R
2
Cog in the
System
How the Limits of Our Brains Leave Us
Vulnerable to Cognitive Hacking
In an attention economy, understanding cognitive psychology gives an informer
(or a disinformer) a major advantage in influencing opinion. What attracts
attention? How do you hold attention? And how, over time, do you manipulate
attention in ways that serve your purposes? These are key questions that we
need to find answers to before we can fully grasp the role technology plays in
influencing opinion.
Clickbait: You Won’t Believe What
Happens Next!
Don’t you hate clickbait? Those online titles like “This Father Found a
Rattlesnake in his Infant’s Crib, and You Won’t Believe What Happened Next!”
or “27 New Dinner Plans that You Need to Try Before You Die!” As cheap
and as cheesy as they sound, these titles are highly engineered, based on
research into the clicking habits of millions of users online. Search something
like “how to write a viral headline,” and you’ll find no limit of marketing how-
to’s telling you to use odd numbers and phrases that create a sense of urgency.
© Kris Shaffer 2019
K. Shaf fer, Data versus Democracy,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4842-4540-8_2
20
Chapter 2 | Cog in the System
Why have these titles (and, for that matter, the articles attached to them)
become so ubiquitous? Clickbait represents, perhaps better than anything
else, life on the modern web. The abundance of free content and the limits of
the attention economy have media outlets competing for clicks and the
advertising dollars they represent. But because even high-quality information
can be readily found with little effort, or money, many media outlets no longer
compete to have the best content, but to have the most attention-grabbing
content.
That’s because in an attention economy, those who thrive aren’t those who
control information—the ones with access to the biggest libraries or the best
content. In an attention economy, those who thrive are those who control
attention—those who can master their own attention and those who can
attract, and hold, the attention of others.
But just what is attention? And why is it that the one, two, or three things
we’re aware of at any given moment can so readily dominate the massive store
of memories we have tucked away in our brains?
Mapping the Cognitive System
Think of your computer. It stores data in several different places—the hard
drive, RAM, the processor’s cache—each with its own function, and with its
characteristic strengths and weaknesses. The hard drive is the largest data
store. Even my 13-inch laptop can hold a terabyte of data on its hard drive.
But compared to other data stores, it is also the slowest and least efficient
when it comes to accessing, writing, and erasing data.
RAM (random access memory), on the other hand, is considerably smaller,
but also faster and more flexible. Because of its power, and its limited size,
applications are constantly vying for access to it, and memory management is
a key element of optimizing the performance of any app or algorithm.
Even smaller—and more powerful—is the cache memory that lives on the
processor itself. This is where the action happens. But it is severely limited
according to what the processor itself is capable of. That’s because data
storage means nothing if you can’t do something with it. The computer’s data
processing capability ultimately sets the limits for what we can do with data.
This hard drive, RAM, cache, processor model is a helpful analog for the
human cognitive system. It’s an oversimplification, of course, but it’s a helpful
starting point, especially if we want to know how the brain interacts with (big)
data.
Data versus Democracy
21
In many ways, the brain is one big hard drive. There’s a reason that we refer
to a computer’s data storage as “memory,” after all. Both the brain and the
hard drive store information in small, hierarchically organized units, which are
activated, transferred, and copied via electrical signals. Some cognitive
scientists even use the language of “bits” to describe the information our
brains process. 1
But rather than having separate organs for large/slow storage, fast/efficient
storage, and data processing, the brain does it all in one massively complex,
and flexible, organ. However, the brain’s capabilities are separated analogously
to how a computer works. (Well, technically, it’s the computer that works like
the brain.) The largest share of the brain’s storage capability is less efficient
and not directly accessible to the “processor.” This is typically called long-term
memory (LTM)—where we keep memories of life events ( episodic memory),
physical skills and processes ( procedural memory), and important information
like the identity of friends or the meaning of words ( semantic memory).2 It’s massive. Each human brain contains the informational complexity of the entire
universe of stars, planets, and galaxies. 3 But we can’t access it all at once.
That’s just too much for the brain to handle. We need something to help us
manage all of that data.
The Limits of Conscious Attention
That’s where working memory comes in. Working memory is the combination
of what scientists call the “central executive” (the brain’s CPU) and several
independent resources for short-term, high-efficiency storage, often called
short-term memory (STM—the brain’s RAM). 4
Think of it this way. Every memory ever formed—every event you’ve
experienced, every word you know, every friend and family member, every
skill you’ve formed—is stored somewhere in your long-term memory (LTM).
Because it takes energy (in the form of electrical signals) to access these
memories, only some of those memories are “activated,” like an application
launched and ready to use, or books taken off the shelf and ready to be read.
1This is based on the “Shannon–Weaver equation,” developed by Claude Shannon and
Warren Weaver in The Mathematical Theory of Communication, Urbana, Ill.: University of
Illinois Press (1949).
2Alan D. Baddeley, Human Memory: Theory and Practice, East Sussex: Psychology Press
(1997), p. 29ff.
3Christof Koch and Patricia Kuhl, “Decoding ‘the Most Complex Object in the Universe’,”
interview by Ira Flatow, Talk of the Nation, NPR, June 14, 2013, www.npr.org/2013/06/
14/191614360/decoding-the-most-complex-object-in-the-universe.
4Alan D. Baddeley, Human Memory: Theory and Practice, East Sussex: Psychology Press
(1997), p. 29ff.
22
Chapter 2 | Cog in the System
This is short-term memory (STM). When something is activated and placed in
STM, it’s only there for a short amount of time. That is, unless we do extra
work to keep it alive. (Cognitive musicologists call that work “rehearsal,” but
it’s actually a lot like renewing a librar
y book before it’s due.) When memories
are in STM, we can do more with them—put them in order, forge relationships
between them, and build higher-level groupings of them (called schemas). But
only some of what we’ve called into STM forms conscious awareness—the
small amount of information being processed right now. That’s the information
that makes up the focus of our attention.
The Triggers of Attention
Attention is incredibly expensive. Such a high level of neural activation, not to
mention the processing that goes on, requires large amounts of energy. That
energy comes at a premium. And so the brain keeps all but a tiny window of
time and data at a lower state of activation, away from the costly power of
consciousness.
Of course, such an efficient and orderly system requires structure—rules that
decide what we pay attention to, and when. When it comes to external
stimuli—sights, sounds, smells—there are many rules of prioritization based
on millennia of natural selection.
As we’ve already discussed, our genetic code was largely written by our
ancient ancestors as they fought for survival.5 Thus, things that meant life or
death on the savannah 150,000 years ago are more likely to command our
attention today. If someone sneaks up behind you and makes a loud, sudden
noise, you’ll jump, suck in extra oxygen (gasp), your hair will stand on end (the
vestige of our ancestors making themselves look bigger and fiercer than they
are), and your heart will speed up (preparing to deliver that extra oxygen to
your muscles, whether they need it for fight or for flight). Perhaps surprisingly,
if they sneak up on you again, but this time they tell you first, some of those
reflexes will still kick in, even though you knew it was coming. Some of our
ancestors survived because these reflexes were so fast and reliable that the
brain could never turn them off. (In fact, some of these reflexes, called “spinal
reflexes, ”6 are directed by the spinal cord, because the speed-of-light trip of
our nerve impulses all the way to the brain and back simply takes too long!)
And those ancestors who survived passed their saber-tooth-tiger-evading
genes onto us.
5Jerome H. Barkow, Leda Cosmides, and John Tooby (eds), The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary
Psychology and the Generation of Culture, New York, NY: Oxford University Press (1992).
6James Knierim, “Spinal Reflexes and Descending Motor Pathways,” in Neuroscience Online,
University of Texas McGovern Medical School, accessed February 8, 2019, https://nba.
uth.tmc.edu/neuroscience/s3/chapter02.html.
Data versus Democracy
23
Other memories—those less critical to our ancestors’ survival—can be
recalled on purpose. Who is that? What’s their phone number? I came into
this room for something, what was it?
Still others feel more natural, more automatic. We may have to actively recall
our new colleague’s name on their second day of work, but it doesn’t take any
work at all to remember our spouse’s, partner’s, or child’s name. That’s
because inside our “big data” brain lies the world’s most advanced predictive
analytics engine. Data we access often remains more “activated” than data
seldom accessed, like the spices that always end up in the front and center of
the cabinet, despite our attempts to alphabetize. Memories that are related
to other memories we’ve already brought to a higher state of activation will
likewise raise in activation. This is why we might not remember that password
or that phone number out of the blue, but when we sit at the keyboard or
pick up the phone, it just comes to us. Cognitive scientists call this priming.
Priming is an essential part of how our brain manages our memories. It
anticipates our needs, delivers what we need in time to respond quickly (just
in case), and it does so while keeping energy usage low. But because priming
is such a key element of how we manage our own attention, it can also be
used by others, in combination with hard-wired evolutionary rules for
processing stimuli, to command our attention and to control, at least in part,
the way we think about the world.
Familiarity Breeds Believability: The Role
of Unconscious Memory
We are constantly evaluating everything we perceive. Is it safe or dangerous?
Is it good or bad? Is it exciting or boring? Did we expect it, or was it a
surprise? These evaluations feed into what we experience as emotions. When
something is surprising and dangerous, the result is fear. If that surprising and
dangerous perception holds our attention for a long time, the result is terror.
If, on the other hand, that surprising thing turns out to be not very dangerous
at all, the result might be laughter—or as cognitive scientist David Huron calls
it, “pleasurable panting” (what you do with all that oxygen your immediate
fear responses sucked in).7 If something is predictable and harmless, the result
might be boredom, especially if that something keeps going on for a long time
(think of a concert or a lecture you found uninteresting, but had no way to
escape). On the other hand, if something is difficult to predict, but also
harmless, the result can be disorientation, and if it goes on for a long time,
7David Huron, Sweet Anticipation: Music and the Psychology of Expectation, Cambridge,
Mass.: The MIT Press (2006), p. 26.
24
Chapter 2 | Cog in the System
frustration.8 This is the core of the creepy-crawly feeling some people get
when listening to avant-garde music, or the repulsion they feel when they
experience other avant-garde art forms. It’s just music, or a painting, or a
sculpture, but the fact that it arrests our attention so strongly, for so long, but
the brain doesn’t know how to make sense of it, that’s a very bad thing,
evolutionarily speaking. And it makes perfect sense that our ancestors would
have evolved an emotional response to such situations that would cause them
to try and avoid them in the future.
Emotion can be a very complex thing. Entire books have been written about
the cognitive science of emotion. But for our purposes in understanding how
humans interact with online media, we’ll cover just a few key elements. Things
that affect our ability (or inability) to recognize disinformation, misinformation,
and “fake news” online, as well as things that promote social polarization.
We’ll start with something simple, yet powerful: the mere exposure effect.
Because of our evolutionary past, we are always assessing whether something
we encounter or perceive—a stimulus—is positive or negative. (Scientists call
the positivity or negativity of a stimulus its valence.) Our life-or-death
evolutionary history means that we make some of these assessments very
quickly. These snap judgments can save our lives, but if they happen
unconsciously in certain social settings, they can form the stuff of
overgeneralizations, stereotypes, and even lead to sexism and racism.
One factor that contributes to a positive affect when we encounter a stimulus
is the ease with which our brain can process
what it perceives—called
perceptual fluency. If the brain can process, understand, and evaluate the
stimulus quickly and without difficulty, the result is a positive affect or emotion.
This is why, for example, notes right next to each other on the piano (and the
cochlea) are “dissonant” rather than forming a pleasant chord. 9 Things that
make it harder for the brain to process—similar colors, sounds, or
appearances—make it hard to distinguish perceived items and place them into
categories, the way the brain likes.
Differences in color, musical pitch, and the like are, for the most part, hard-
wired differences. We may be able through artistic and musical training to
make finer judgments than the average person, but not much finer. There are
physical limitations in our inner ear and on our retina that determine how
finely we can develop our perceptual abilities.
But other differences are learned. We learn language, bodily gestures, faces,
and skills through repeated exposure. Because the brain conserves energy by
8Patrick Colm Hogan, Cognitive Science, Literature, and the Arts, New York: Routledge
(2003), 9–11.
9R. Plomp and J. M. Levelt, “Tonal Consonance and Critical Bandwidth,” in Journal of the
Acoustical Society of America 37 (1965), pp. 548–60.
Data versus Democracy
25
preactivating parts of memory that it anticipates us needing, and because it’s
more likely to anticipate something that we encounter regularly than
something we encounter rarely (let alone something entirely new), the brain
preactivates the resources needed to parse familiar things far more readily
than the resources needed to parse unfamiliar things. As a result, the brain
processes familiar things faster and more easily than it processes unfamiliar
things. Finally, since fast, easy processing is associated with a positive reaction,
familiar things tend to lead to more positive emotions than unfamiliar things,
all else being equal.
This makes sense. When a piece of music ends the way we might expect, that
feels good. No surprises. Everything is in its right place. And that’s true even
when we don’t know enough music theory to explain what “should” happen