Book Read Free

What Intelligence Tests Miss

Page 36

by Keith E Stanovich


  Reyna, V. F., Lloyd, F. J., & Brainerd, C. J. (2003). Memory, development, and rationality: An integrative theory of judgment and decision making. In S. L. Schneider & J. Shanteau (Eds.), Emerging perspectives on judgment and decision research (201–245). New York: Cambridge University Press.

  Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

  Ridley, M. (2000). Mendel’s demon: Gene justice and the complexity of life. London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson.

  Ritchhart, R., & Perkins, D. N. (2005). Learning to think: The challenges of teaching thinking. In K. J. Holyoak & R. G. Morrison (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning (775–802). New York: Cambridge University Press.

  Rodriguez, M. L., Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1989). Cognitive person variables in delay of gratification of older children at risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 358–367.

  Roese, N. (1997). Counterfactual thinking. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 131–148.

  Rokeach, M. (1960). The open and closed mind. New York: Basic Books.

  Ronis, D. L., & Yates, J. F. (1987). Components of probability judgment accuracy: Individual consistency and effects of subject matter and assessment method. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 40, 193–218.

  Roseman, I. J., & Read, S. J. (2007). Psychologist at play: Robert Abelson’s life and contributions to psychological science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 86–97.

  Rottenstreich, Y., & Hsee, C. K. (2001). Money, kisses, and electric shocks: On the affective psychology of risk. Psychological Science, 12, 185–190.

  Royal swedish academy of Sciences (2002a). Advanced information on the Prize in Economic Sciences 2002. Retrieved August 6, 2007 from nobelprize.org/nobel_prizes/economics/laureates/2002/ecoadv02.pdf.

  Royal Swedish Academy of Sciences (2002b). Press Release: The Bank of Sweden Prize in Economic Sciences in Memory of Alfred Nobel 2002 Information for the Public. Retrieved August 6, 2007 from http://www.nobel.se/economics/laureates/2002/press.html.

  Royzman, E. B., Cassidy, K. W., & Baron, J. (2003). “I know, you know”: Epistemic egocentrism in children and adults. Review of General Psychology, 7, 38–65.

  Rozin, P., Kabnick, K., Pete, E., Fischler, C., & Shields, C. (2003). The ecology of eating: Smaller portions in France than in the United States help explain the French paradox. Psychological Science, 14, 450–454.

  Rumelhart, D. E., Smolensky, P., McClelland, J. L., & Hinton, G. E. (1986). Schemata and sequential thought processes in PDP models. In J. L. McClelland & D. E. Rumelhart (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition (Vol. 2) (7–57). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  Russo, J. E., & Schoemaker, P. (1989). Decision traps: Ten barriers to brilliant decision making and how to overcome them. New York: simon & Schuster.

  Sá, W., Kelley, C., Ho, C., & Stanovich, K. E. (2005). Thinking about personal theories: Individual differences in the coordination of theory and evidence. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 1149–1161.

  Sá, W., & Stanovich, K. E. (2001). The domain specificity and generality of mental contamination: Accuracy and projection in judgments of mental content. British Journal of Psychology, 92, 281–302.

  Sá, W., West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (1999). The domain specificity and generality of belief bias: Searching for a generalizable critical thinking skill. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 497–510.

  Sabini, J., & Silver, M. (1998). Emotion, character, and responsibility. New York: Oxford University Press.

  Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274, 1926–1928.

  Sailer, S. (2004, October 21). This just in—Kerry’s IQ likely lower than Bush’s! Retrieved July 16, 2007 from http://www.vdare.com/sailer/kerry_iq_lower.htm.

  Salthouse, T. A., Atkinson, T. M., & Berish, D. E. (2003). Executive functioning as a potential mediator of age-related cognitive decline in normal adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 566–594.

  Samuels, R. (2005). The complexity of cognition: Tractability arguments for massive modularity. In P. Carruthers, S. Laurence, & S. Stich (Eds.), The innate mind (107–121). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  Samuels, R. (2008). The magical number two, plus or minus: Some comments on dual-process theories. In J. Evans & K. Frankish (Eds.), In two minds: Dual processes and beyond. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  Samuels, R., & Stich, S. P. (2004). Rationality and psychology. In A. R. Mele & P. Rawling (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of rationality (279–300). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  Samuelson, W., & Zeckhauser, R. J. (1988). Status quo bias in decision making. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 1, 7–59.

  Sanna, L. J., & Schwarz, N. (2004). Integrating temporal biases: The interplay of focal thoughts and accessibility experiences. Psychological Science, 15, 474–481.

  Sanna, L. J., & Schwarz, N. (2006). Metacognitive experiences and human judgment: The case of hindsight bias and its debiasing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 172–176.

  Savage, L. J. (1954). The foundations of statistics. New York: Wiley.

  Schaefer, P. S., Williams, C. C., Goodie, A. S., & Campbell, W. K. (2004). Over-confidence and the Big Five. Journal of Research in Personality, 38, 473–480.

  Schelling, T. C. (1984). Choice and consequence: Perspectives of an errant economist. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  Schkade, D. A., & Kahneman, D. (1998). Does living in California make people happy? Psychological Science, 9, 340–346.

  Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1992). Development of a causal model of processes determining job performance. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1, 89–92.

  Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1998). The validity and utility of selection methods in personnel psychology: Practical and theoretical implications of 85 years of research findings. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 262–274.

  Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (2004). General mental ability in the world of work: Occupational attainment and job performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 162–173.

  Schneider, S. L. (1995). Item difficulty, discrimination, and the confidence frequency effect in a categorical judgment task. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 61, 148–167.

  Schneider, S. L., Burke, M. D., Solomonson, A. L., & Laurion, S. K. (2005). Incidental framing effects and associative processes: A study of attribute frames in broadcast news stories. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 18, 261–280.

  Schneider, S. L., & Shanteau, J. (Eds.) (2003). Emerging perspectives on judgment and decision research. New York: Cambridge University Press.

  Schommer, M. (1990). Effects of beliefs about the nature of knowledge on comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 498–504.

  Schooler, C. (1998). Environmental complexity and the Flynn effect. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The rising curve: Long-term changes in IQ and related measures (67–79). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

  Schwartz, B. (2004). The paradox of choice. New York: Ecco Press.

  Schwarz, N. (1996). Cognition and communication: Judgmental biases, research methods, and the logic of conversation. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

  Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2003). Mood as information: 20 years later. Psychological Inquiry, 14, 296–303.

  Schweizer, K., & Koch, W. (2002). A revision of Cattell’s Investment Theory: Cognitive properties influencing learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 13, 57–82.

  Sedlmeier, P. (1999). Improving statistical reasoning: Theoretical models and practical implications. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

  Sedlmeier, P., & Gigerenzer, G. (2001). Teaching Bayesian reasoning in less than two hours. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 380–400.

  Seligmann, J., & Chideya, F. (1992, September 21).
Horror story or big hoax? Newsweek, 75.

  Shafir, E. (1994). Uncertainty and the difficulty of thinking through disjunctions. Cognition, 50, 403–430.

  Shafir, E. (Ed.) (2003). Preference, belief, and similarity: Selected writings of Amos Tversky. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  Shafir, E., Diamond, P., & Tversky, A. (1997). Money illusion. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 112, 341–374.

  Shafir, E., & Leboeuf, R. A. (2002). Rationality. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 491–517.

  Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  Shanks, D. R. (1995). Is human learning rational? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48a, 257–279.

  Shermer, M. (1997). Why people believe weird things. New York: W. H. Freeman.

  Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127–190.

  Shiv, B., Loewenstein, G., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (2005). Investment behavior and the negative side of emotion. Psychological Science, 16, 435–439.

  Showalter, E. (1997). Hystories: Hysterical epidemics and modern media. New York: columbia University Press.

  Sieck, W. R., & Arkes, H. R. (2005). The recalcitrance of overconfidence and its contribution to decision aid neglect. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 18, 29–53.

  Simon, A. F., Fagley, N. S., & Halleran, J. G. (2004). Decision framing: Moderating effects of individual differences and cognitive processing. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 17, 77–93.

  Simon, H. A. (1955). A behavioral model of rational choice. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 69, 99–118.

  Simon, H. A. (1956). Rational choice and the structure of the environment. Psychological Review, 63, 129–138.

  Simoneau, M., & Markovits, H. (2003). Reasoning with premises that are not empirically true: Evidence for the role of inhibition and retrieval. Developmental Psychology, 39, 964–975.

  Simonton, D. K. (2006). Presidential IQ, openness, intellectual brilliance, and leadership: Estimates and correlations for 42 U.S. chief executives. Political Psychology, 27, 511–526.

  Sinaceur, M., Heath, C., & Cole, S. (2005). Emotional and deliberative reactions to a public crisis: Mad Cow Disease in France. Psychological Science, 16, 247–254.

  Sivak, M. (2002). How common sense fails us on the road: Contribution of bounded rationality to the annual worldwide toll of one million traffic fatalities. Transportation Research Part F, 5, 259–269.

  Sivak, M., & Flannagan, M. J. (2003). Flying and driving after the September 11 attacks. American Scientist, 91, 6–7.

  Skyrms, B. (1996). The evolution of the social contract. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  Slemrod, J., & Bakija, J. (1996). Taxing ourselves: A citizen’s guide to the great debate over tax reform. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  Sloman, A. (1993). The mind as a control system. In C. Hookway & D. Peterson (Eds.), Philosophy and cognitive science (69–110). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  Sloman, A., & Chrisley, R. (2003). Virtual machines and consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10, 133–172.

  Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 3–22.

  Sloman, S. A. (2002). Two systems of reasoning. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment (379–396). New York: Cambridge University Press.

  Sloman, S. A., & Over, D. E. (2003). Probability judgement from the inside out. In D. E. Over (Ed.), Evolution and the psychology of thinking (145–169). Hove, England: Psychology Press.

  Sloman, S. A., Over, D., Slovak, L., & Stibel, J. M. (2003). Frequency illusions and other fallacies. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 91, 296–309.

  Slovic, P. (1995). The construction of preference. American Psychologist, 50, 364–371.

  Slovic, P. (2007). “If I look at the mass I will never act”: Psychic numbing and genocide. Judgment and Decision Making, 2, 79–95.

  Slovic, P., Finucane, M. L., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2002). The affect heuristic. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment (397–420). New York: Cambridge University Press.

  Slovic, P., Monahan, J., & MacGregor, D. G. (2000). Violence risk assessment and risk communication: The effects of using actual cases, providing instruction, and employing probability versus frequency formats. Law and Human Behavior, 24, 271–296.

  Slovic, P., & Peters, E. (2006). Risk perception and affect. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 322–325.

  Slugoski, B. R., & Wilson, A. E. (1998). Contribution of conversation skills to the production of judgmental errors. European Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 575–601.

  Smith, E. E., Patalino, A. L., & Jonides, J. (1998). Alternative strategies of categorization. In S. A. Sloman & L. J. Rips (Eds.), Similarity and symbols in human thinking (81–110). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  Smith, E. R., & DeCoster, J. (2000). Dual-process models in social and cognitive psychology: Conceptual integration and links to underlying memory systems. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 108–131.

  Smith, M., & Belcher, R. (1993). Facilitated communication with adults with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23, 175–183.

  Snow, D. N., Zurcher, L. A., & Ekland-Olson, S. (1980). Social networks and social movements: A microstructural approach to differential recruitment. American Sociological Review, 45, 787–801.

  Sonuga-Barke, E. (2002). Psychological heterogeneity in AD/HD—a dual pathway model of behavior and cognition. Behavioural Brain Research, 130, 29–36.

  Sonuga-Barke, E. (2003). The dual pathway model of AD/HD: An elaboration of neurodevelopmental characteristics. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 27, 593–604.

  Spearman, C. (1904). General intelligence, objectively determined and measured. American Journal of Psychology, 15, 201–293.

  Sperber, D. (1994). The modularity of thought and the epidemiology of representations. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (39–67). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. Oxford: Blackwell.

  Sperber, D. (2000). Metarepresentations in evolutionary perspective. In D. Sperber (Ed.), Metarepresentations: A multidisciplinary perspective (117–137). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  Sperber, D., Cara, F., & Girotto, V. (1995). Relevance theory explains the selection task. Cognition, 57, 31–95.

  Spitz, H. H. (1997). Nonconscious movements: From mystical messages to facilitated communication. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Stanovich, K. E. (1993a). Dysrationalia: A new specific learning disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26, 501–515.

  Stanovich, K. E. (1993b). It’s practical to be rational. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26, 524–532.

  Stanovich, K. E. (1994a). Reconceptualizing intelligence: Dysrationalia as an intuition pump. Educational Researcher, 23(4), 11–22.

  Stanovich, K. E. (1994b). The evolving concept of rationality. Educational Researcher, 23(7), 33.

  Stanovich, K. E. (1999). Who is rational? Studies of individual differences in reasoning. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

  Stanovich, K. E. (2000). Progress in understanding reading: Scientific foundations and new frontiers. New York: Guilford Press.

  Stanovich, K. E. (2001a). Reductionism in the study of intelligence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(2), 91–92.

  Stanovich, K. E. (2001b). The rationality of educating for wisdom. Educational Psychologist, 36, 247–251.

  Stanovich, K. E. (2004). The robot’s rebellion: Finding meaning in the age of Darwin. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

  Stanovich, K. E. (2005). The future of a mistake: W
ill discrepancy measurement continue to make the learning disabilities field a pseudoscience? Learning Disability Quarterly, 28, 103–106.

  Stanovich, K. E. (2007). How to think straight about psychology (8th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

  Stanovich, K. E. (2008). Distinguishing the reflective, algorithmic, and autonomous minds: Is it time for a tri-process theory? In J. Evans & K. Frankish (Eds.), In two minds: Dual processes and beyond. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  Stanovich, K. E. (2009). Rationality and the reflective mind: Toward a tri-process model of cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.

  Stanovich, K. E., Grunewald, M., & West, R. F. (2003). Cost-benefit reasoning in students with multiple secondary school suspensions. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 1061–1072.

  Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1994). Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 24– 53.

  Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1997). Reasoning independently of prior belief and individual differences in actively open-minded thinking. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 342–357.

  Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1998a). Cognitive ability and variation in selection task performance. Thinking and Reasoning, 4, 193–230.

  Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1998b). Individual differences in framing and conjunction effects. Thinking and Reasoning, 4, 289–317.

  Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1998c). Individual differences in rational thought. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 161–188.

  Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1998d). Who uses base rates and P(D/~H)? An analysis of individual differences. Memory & Cognition, 26, 161–179.

  Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1999). Discrepancies between normative and descriptive models of decision making and the understanding/acceptance principle. Cognitive Psychology, 38, 349–385.

  Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 645–726.

  Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2007). Natural myside bias is independent of cognitive ability. Thinking and Reasoning, 13, 225–247.

 

‹ Prev