What Intelligence Tests Miss
Page 37
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2008a). On the failure of intelligence to predict myside bias and one-sided bias. Thinking and Reasoning, 14, 129–167.
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2008b). On the relative independence of thinking biases and cognitive ability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 672–695.
Stein, E. (1996). Without good reason: The rationality debate in philosophy and cognitive science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Stenger, V. J. (1990). Physics and psychics. Buffalo, NY: Prometheus Books.
Stenning, K., & van Lambalgen, M. (2004). The natural history of hypotheses about the selection task. In K. I. Manktelow & M. C. Chung (Eds.), Psychology of reasoning (127–156). Hove, England: Psychology Press.
Sterelny, K. (1990). The representational theory of mind: An introduction. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Sterelny, K. (2001). The evolution of agency and other essays. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Sterelny, K. (2003). Thought in a hostile world: The evolution of human cognition. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Beyond IQ: A triarchic theory of human intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Sternberg, R. J. (1988). The triarchic mind. New York: Viking.
Sternberg, R. J. (1993). Would you rather take orders from Kirk or Spock? The relation between rational thinking and intelligence. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26, 516–519.
Sternberg, R. J. (1994). What if the concept of dysrationalia were an example of itself? Educational Researcher, 23 (4), 22–23.
Sternberg, R. J. (1997a). Successful intelligence: How practical and creative intelligence determine success in life. New York: Plume.
Sternberg, R. J. (1997b). The concept of intelligence and its role in lifelong learning and success. American Psychologist, 52, 1030–1037.
Sternberg, R. J. (1997c). Thinking styles. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.) (2000a). Handbook of intelligence. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Sternberg, R. J. (2000b). The concept of intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (3–15). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Sternberg, R. J. (2001). Why schools should teach for wisdom: The balance theory of wisdom in educational settings. Educational Psychologist, 36, 227–245.
Sternberg, R. J. (2002a). Smart people are not stupid, but they sure can be foolish: The imbalance theory of foolishness. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Why smart people can be so stupid (232–242). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.) (2002b). Why smart people can be so stupid. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Sternberg, R. J. (2003a). Issues in the theory and measurement of successful intelligence: A reply to Brody. Intelligence, 31, 331–337.
Sternberg, R. J. (2003b). Wisdom, intelligence, and creativity synthesized. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Sternberg, R. J. (2004). Theory-based university admissions testing for a new millennium. Educational Psychologist, 39, 185–198.
Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.) (2005). The psychology of hate. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Sternberg, R. J. (2006). The Rainbow Project: Enhancing the SAT through assessments of analytical, practical, and creative skills. Intelligence, 34, 321–350.
Sternberg, R. J., Conway, B., Ketron, J., & Bernstein, M. (1981). People’s conceptions of intelligence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 37–55.
Sternberg, R. J., & Detterman, D. K. (Eds.) (1986). What is intelligence? Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Sternberg, R. J., & Grigorenko, E. L. (1997). Are cognitive styles still in style? American Psychologist, 52, 700–712.
Sternberg, R. J., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2004). Why we need to explore development in its cultural context. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 50, 369–386.
Sternberg, R. J., & Kaufman, J. C. (1998). Human abilities. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 479–502.
Sternberg, R. J., & Ruzgis, P. (Eds.) (1994). Personality and intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Stich, S. P. (1990). The fragmentation of reason. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Stigler, S. M. (1983). Who discovered Bayes’s theorem? American Statistician, 37, 290–296.
Stigler, S. M. (1986). The history of statistics: The measurement of uncertainty before 1900. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Strathman, A., Gleicher, F., Boninger, D. S., & Scott Edwards, C. (1994). The consideration of future consequences: Weighing immediate and distant outcomes of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 742–752.
Strayer, D. L., & Drews, F. A. (2007). Cell-phone-induced driver distraction. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 128–131.
Strayer, D. L., & Johnston, W. A. (2001). Driven to distraction: Dual-task studies of simulated driving and conversing on a cellular telephone. Psychological Science, 12, 462–466.
Stroud, S., & Tappolet, C. (Eds.) (2003). Weakness of will and practical irrationality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Stuebing, K., Fletcher, J. M., LeDoux, J. M., Lyon, G. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2002). Validity of IQ-discrepancy classification of reading difficulties: A meta-analysis. American Educational Research Journal, 39, 469–518.
Sub, H.-M., Oberauer, K., Wittmann, W. W., Wilhelm, O., & Schulze, R. (2002). Working-memory capacity explains reasoning ability—and a little bit more. Intelligence, 30, 261–288.
Suddendorf, T. (1999). The rise of the metamind. In M. C. Corballis & S. Lea (Eds.), The descent of mind: Psychological perspectives on hominid evolution (218–260). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 299–351.
Suddendorf, T., & Whiten, A. (2001). Mental evolution and development: Evidence for secondary representation in children, great apes, and other animals. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 629–650.
Sunstein, C. R. (2002). Risk and reason: Safety, law, and the environment. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Sunstein, C. R. (2005). Moral heuristics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 531–573.
Sunstein, C. R., & Thaler, R. H. (2003). Libertarian paternalism is not an oxymoron. University of Chicago Law Review, 70, 1159–1202.
Surowiecki, J. (2007, May 28). Feature presentation. The New Yorker, 28.
Suskind, R. (2004). The price of loyalty. New York: simon & Schuster.
Suskind, R. (2006). The one percent doctrine. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Svenson, O. (1981). Are we all less risky and more skillful than our fellow drivers? Acta Psychologica, 47, 143–148.
Swartz, R. J., & Perkins, D. N. (1989). Teaching thinking: Issues & approaches. Pacific Grove, CA: Midwest Publications.
Taber, C. S., & Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science, 50, 755–769.
Taleb, N. (2001). Fooled by randomness. New York: Texere.
Taleb, N. (2007). The black swan: The impact of the highly improbable. New York: Random House.
Tannock, R. (1998). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Advances in cognitive, neurobiological, and genetic research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 65–99.
Taub, G. E., & McGrew, K. S. (2004). A confirmatory factor analysis of Cattellhorn-Carroll theory and cross-age invariance of the Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities III. School Psychology Quarterly, 19, 72–87.
Taylor, S. E. (1981). The interface of cognitive and social psychology. In J. H. Harvey (Ed.), Cognition, social behavior, and the environment (189–211). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Tetlock, P. E. (2005). Expert political judgment. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Thagard, P. (2006). Hot thought: Mechanisms and applications of emotional cognition. Cambridge, M
A: MIT Press.
Thagard, P., & Nisbett, R. E. (1983). Rationality and charity. Philosophy of Science, 50, 250–267.
Thaler, R. H. (1980). Toward a positive theory of consumer choice. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 1, 39–60.
Thaler, R. H., & Benartzi, S. (2004). Save more tomorrow: Using behavioral economics to increase employee saving. Journal of Political Economy, 112, S164–S187.
Thaler, R. H., Tversky, A., Kahneman, D., & Schwartz, A. (1997). The effect of myopia and loss aversion on risk taking: An experimental test. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 112, 647–661.
Thomas, E., & Wolffe, R. (2005, December 19). Bush in the bubble. Newsweek, 31–39.
Thomson, J. J. (1976). Killing, letting die, and the trolley problem. The Monist, 59, 204–217.
Thomson, J. J. (1985). The trolley problem. Yale Law Journal, 94, 1395–1415.
Thomson, J. J. (1990). The realm of rights. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Thurstone, L. L. (1927). The nature of intelligence. New York: Harcourt, Brace.
Todd, P. M., & Gigerenzer, G. (2000). Precis of simple heuristics that make us smart. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 727–780.
Todd, P. M., & Gigerenzer, G. (2007). Environments that make us smart: Ecological rationality. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 167–171.
Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: harvard University Press.
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind (19–136). New York: Oxford University Press.
Toplak, M., liu, E., Macpherson, R., Toneatto, T., & Stanovich, K. E. (2007). The reasoning skills and thinking dispositions of problem gamblers: A dualprocess taxonomy. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 20, 103–124.
Toplak, M. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (2002). The domain specificity and generality of disjunctive reasoning: Searching for a generalizable critical thinking skill. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 197–209.
Toplak, M. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (2003). Associations between myside bias on an informal reasoning task and amount of post-secondary education. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 851–860.
Torrey, E. F. (1984). The roots of treason. San Diego: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Trends in the Well Being of America’s Children & Youth, 2003 (2003). U.S. Department of Health and Human Services Office of the Assistant Secretary for Planning and Evaluation, http://aspe.hhs.gov/.
Tversky, A. (1996). Contrasting rational and psychological principles of choice. In R. Zeckhauser, R. Keeney, & J. Sebenius (Eds.), Wise choices (5–21). Boston: Harvard Business School Press.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453–458.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1982). Evidential impact of base rates. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (153–160). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1983). Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: The conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychological Review, 90, 293–315.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1986). Rational choice and the framing of decisions. Journal of Business, 59, 251–278.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1992). Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation under uncertainty. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 5, 297–323.
Tversky, A., & Shafir, E. (1992). The disjunction effect in choice under uncertainty. Psychological Science, 3, 305–309.
Twachtman-Cullen, D. (1997). A passion to believe. Boulder, CO: Westview.
Tweney, R. D., Doherty, M. E., Warner, W. J., & Pliske, D. (1980). Strategies of rule discovery in an inference task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 109–124.
Ubel, P. A. (2000). Pricing life: Why it’s time for health care rationing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Unger, P. (1996). Living high & letting die: Our illusion of innocence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Working memory capacity and fluid abilities: Examining the correlation between Operation Span and Raven. Intelligence, 33, 67–81.
Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. W. (2007). The nature of individual differences in working memory capacity: Active maintenance in primary memory and controlled search from secondary memory. Psychological Review, 114, 104–132.
U.S. Congress House Select Committee on Aging (1984, May 31). Quackery: A $10 billion scandal. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Valentine, D. A. (1998, May 13). Pyramid schemes. Presented at the International Monetary Fund Seminar on Current Legal Issues Affecting Central Banks. Washington, DC: IMF. Retrieved from http://www.ftc.gov/speeches/other/dvimf16.shtm on August 29, 2007.
Valentine, E. R. (1975). Performance on two reasoning tasks in relation to intelligence, divergence and interference proneness: Content and context effects in reasoning. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 45, 198–205.
Vallone, R., Griffin, D. W., Lin, S., & Ross, L. (1990). Overconfident prediction of future actions and outcomes by self and others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 582–592.
Vellutino, F., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 2–40.
Vinter, A., & Detable, C. (2003). Implicit learning in children and adolescents with mental retardation. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 108, 94–107.
Vinter, A., & Perruchet, P. (2000). Implicit learning in children is not related to age: Evidence from drawing behavior. Child Development, 71, 1223–1240.
Visser, B. A., Ashton, M. C., & Vernon, P. A. (2006). Beyond g: Putting multiple intelligences theory to the test. Intelligence, 34, 487–502.
von Neumann, J., & Morgenstern, O. (1944). The theory of games and economic behavior. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Wagenaar, W. A. (1988). Paradoxes of gambling behavior. Hove, England: Erlbaum.
Waldmann, M. R., & Dietrich, J. H. (2007). Throwing a bomb on a person versus throwing a person on a bomb. Psychological Science, 18, 247–253.
Wang, P. (2006, October). What works in retirement planning. Money Magazine, 124–130.
Wason, P. C. (1960). On the failure to eliminate hypotheses in a conceptual task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12, 129–140.
Wason, P. C. (1966). Reasoning. In B. Foss (Ed.), New horizons in psychology (135–151). Harmondsworth, England: Penguin.
Wason, P. C. (1968). Reasoning about a rule. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 273–281.
Wason, P. C. (1969). Regression in reasoning? British Journal of Psychology, 60, 471–480.
Wasserman, E. A., Dorner, W. W., & Kao, S. F. (1990). Contributions of specific cell information to judgments of interevent contingency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 509–521.
Waterhouse, L. (2006). Multiple intelligences, the Mozart effect, and emotional intelligence: A critical review. Educational Psychologist, 41, 207–226.
Watkins, S. J. (2000). Conviction by mathematical error? British Medical Journal, 320(7226), 2–3.
Wechsler, D. (1958). The measurement and appraisal of adult intelligence. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.
Wegner, D. M. (2002). The illusion of conscious will. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Wegner, D. M., Fuller, V. A., & Sparrow, B. (2003). Clever hands: Uncontrolled intelligence in facilitated communication. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 5–19.
Wertenbroch, K., Soman, D., & Chattopadhyay, A. (2007). On the perceived value of money: The reference dependence of currency numerosity effects. Journal of C
onsumer Research, 34, 1–10.
West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (1997). The domain specificity and generality of overconfidence: Individual differences in performance estimation bias. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 387–392.
West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (2003). Is probability matching smart? Associations between probabilistic choices and cognitive ability. Memory & Cognition, 31, 243–251.
Westen, D., Blagov, P., Kilts, C., & Hamann, S. (2006). Neural Bases of motivated reasoning: An fMRI study of emotional constraints on partisan political judgment in the 2004 U.S. presidential election. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1947–1958.
Whittington, D. (1991). What have 17-year-olds known in the past? American Educational Research Journal, 28, 759–780.
Wilkinson, P. (1998, July 9). Juror who wanted to find truth in the stars. Times (London).
Will, G. F. (2005, October 5). Can this nomination be justified? Washington Post, A23.
Williams, W. M. (1998). Are we raising smarter children today? School and home-related influences on IQ. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The rising curve: Long-term changes in IQ and related measures (125–154). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Willingham, D. T. (1998). A neuropsychological theory of motor-skill learning. Psychological Review, 105, 558–584.
Willingham, D. T. (1999). The neural basis of motor-skill learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 178–182.
Willingham, D. T. (2004). Reframing the mind. Education Next, 4(3), 19–24.
Wilson, T. D. (2002). Strangers to ourselves. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2005). Affective forecasting: Knowing what to want. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 131–134.
Wilson, T. D., Houston, C. E., Etling, K. M., & Brekke, N. (1996). A new look at anchoring effects: Basic anchoring and its antecedents. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 387–402.
Wilson, T. D., Wheatley, T., Meyers, J. M., Gilbert, D. T., & Axsom, D. (2000). Focalism: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 821–836.